Что означает memory error

Memory gaps and errors refer to the incorrect recall, or complete loss, of information in the memory system for a specific detail and/or event. Memory errors may include remembering events that never occurred, or remembering them differently from the way they actually happened.[1] These errors or gaps can occur due to a number of different reasons, including the emotional involvement in the situation, expectations and environmental changes. As the retention interval between encoding and retrieval of the memory lengthens, there is an increase in both the amount that is forgotten, and the likelihood of a memory error occurring.

Memory gaps and errors refer to the incorrect recall, or complete loss, of information in the memory system for a specific detail and/or event. Memory errors may include remembering events that never occurred, or remembering them differently from the way they actually happened.[1] These errors or gaps can occur due to a number of different reasons, including the emotional involvement in the situation, expectations and environmental changes. As the retention interval between encoding and retrieval of the memory lengthens, there is an increase in both the amount that is forgotten, and the likelihood of a memory error occurring.

OverviewEdit

There are several different types of memory errors, in which people may inaccurately recall details of events that did not occur, or they may simply misattribute the source of a memory. In other instances, imagination of a certain event can create confidence that such an event actually occurred. Causes of such memory errors may be due to certain cognitive factors, such as spreading activation, or to physiological factors, including brain damage, age or emotional factors. Furthermore, memory errors have been reported in individuals with schizophrenia and depression. The consequences of memory errors can have significant implications. Two main areas of concern regarding memory errors are in eyewitness testimony and cases of child abuse.

Types of memory errorsEdit

BlockingEdit

The feeling that a person gets when they know the information, but can not remember a specific detail, like an individual’s name or the name of a place is described as the «tip-of-the-tongue» experience. The tip-of-the-tongue experience is a classic example of blocking, which is a failure to retrieve information that is available in memory even though you are trying to produce it.[2] The information you are trying to remember has been encoded and stored, and a cue is available that would usually trigger its recollection.[2] The information has not faded from memory and a person is not forgetting to retrieve the information.[2] What a person is experiencing is a complete retrieval failure, which makes blocking especially frustrating.[2] Blocking occurs especially often for the names of people and places, because their links to related concepts and knowledge are weaker than for common names.[2] The experience of blocking occurs more often as we get older; this «tip of the tongue» experience is a common complaint amongst 60- and 70-year-olds.[2]

TransienceEdit

Transience refers to forgetting what occurs with the passage of time.[3] Transience occurs during the storage phase of memory, after an experience has been encoded and before it is retrieved.[3] As time passes, the quality of our memory also changes, deteriorating from specific to more general.[3] German philosopher named Hermann Ebbinghaus decided to measure his own memory for lists of nonsense syllables at various times after studying them. He decided to draw out a curve of his forgetting pattern over time. He realized that there is a rapid drop-off in retention during the first tests and there is a slower rate of forgetting later on.[3] Therefore, transience denotes the gradual change of a specific knowledge or idea into more general memories.[4]

AbsentmindednessEdit

Absentmindedness is a gap in attention which causes memory failure. In this situation the information does not disappear from memory, it can later be recalled. But the lack of attention at a specific moment prevents the information from being recalled at that specific moment. A common cause of absentmindedness is a lack of attention.[clarification needed] Attention is vital to encoding information in long-term memory. Without proper attention, material is much less likely to be stored properly and recalled later.[5] When attention is divided, less activity in the lower left frontal lobe diminishes the ability for elaborative memory encoding to take place, and results in absentminded forgetting. More recent research has shown that divided attention also leads to less hippocampal involvement in encoding.[5] A common example of absentmindedness is not remembering to carry out actions that had been planned to be done in the future, for example, picking up grocery items, and remembering times of meetings.[6]

False memoriesEdit

False memories, sometimes referred to as confabulation, refer to the recollection of inaccurate details of an event, or recollection of a whole event that never occurred. Studies investigating this memory error have been able to successfully implant memories among participants that never existed, such as being lost in a mall as a child (termed the lost in the mall technique) or spilling a bowl of punch at a wedding reception.[7] In this case, false memories were implanted among participants by their family members who claimed that the event had happened. This evidence demonstrates the possibility of implanting false memories on individuals by leading them to remember such events that never occurred. This memory error can be particularly worrisome in judicial settings, in which witnesses may have false recollections of a crime after it has happened, especially when told by others that particular things may have happened which did not.[8] Another area of concern regarding false memories is in cases of child abuse.

Problem of biasEdit

The problem of bias, which is the distorting influences of present knowledge, beliefs, and feelings on recollection of previous experiences.[9] Sometimes what people remember from their past says less about what actually happened than about what they personally believe, feel, and the knowledge they have acquired at the present time.[9] An individual’s current moods can bias their memory recall, researchers have found.[9] There are three types of memory biases, consistency bias, change bias and egocentric bias.[9] Consistency bias is the bias to reconstruct the past to fit the present.[9] Change bias is the tendency to exaggerate differences between what we feel or believe in the present and what we previously felt or believed in the past.[9] Egocentric bias is a form of change bias, the tendency to exaggerate the change between the past and the present in order to make ourselves look good in any given situation.[9]

Misinformation effectEdit

The misinformation effect refers to the change in memory due to the presentation of information that is relevant to the target memory, such as leading questions or suggestions.[10] Memories are likely to be altered when questions are worded differently or when inaccurate information is presented. For example, in one experiment participants watched a video of an automobile accident and were then asked questions regarding the accident. When asked how fast the automobiles were driving when they smashed into each other, the speed estimate was higher than when asked how fast the automobiles were driving when they hit, bumped or collided into each other. Similarly, participants were more likely to report there being shattered glass present when the word smashed was used instead of other verbs.[11] Evidently, memory recollection can be altered with misleading information after the event.

Source confusionEdit

Source confusion or unconscious transference,[12] involves the misattribution of the source of a memory. For instance, an individual may recall seeing an event in person when in reality they only witnessed the event on television.[12] Ultimately, the individual has an inability to remember the source of information in which the content and the source become dissociated. This may be more likely for more distant memories, such as childhood memories.[7] In more severe cases of source confusion, you can take fictional stories you heard from when you were younger and assimilated these stories being your childhood. For example, say your father told you stories about his life when he was a child every night before you went to sleep when you were a child. When you grow up, you might mistakenly remember these stories your dad told you as your own and integrate them into your childhood memories.[13]

Imagination inflationEdit

Imagination inflation refers to when a person remembers details of a memory that are exaggerated versions of the actual event or remember an entire memory that never occurred due to the act of imagination.[14] That is, when one imagines an event occurring, their confidence that this event actually did occur increases. One reason for this may be due to the act of imagination increasing the familiarity of the event. When the event seems more familiar, it may become more likely for people to report it actually occurring. For instance, in an experiment participants were asked to imagine playing inside and then running outside toward a window, falling and breaking it, while other participants did not imagine anything. Participants who had imagined this scenario reported an increased level of confidence that the event had actually happened in comparison to those who did not imagine the event.[7] This error can be caused simply by imagining an event.

Deese–Roediger–McDermott (DRM) paradigmEdit

Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm refers to the incorrect recall of features of an event that were not actually present, due to the features being related to a common theme.[15] This paradigm has been demonstrated with the use of word lists and subsequent recognition tests. For example, experiments have shown that if a research participant is presented with the words: bed rest awake tired dream wake snooze snore nap yawn drowsy, there is a high likelihood that the participant will falsely recall that the word sleep was in the list of words. These results show a significant illusion in memory, in which people remember items that were never presented simply due to their relation with other items in a common theme.[1]

Schematic errorsEdit

Schematic errors refer to the use of a schema to help reconstruct parts of an experience that cannot be remembered. This may include parts of the schema that did not actually take place, or aspects of a schema that are stereotypical of an event.[16] Schemas can be described as mental guidelines (scripts) for events that are encountered in daily life.[16] For example, when going to the gas station, there is a general pattern of how things will occur (i.e. turn car off, get out of car, open gas tank, punch the gas button, put nozzle into the tank, fill up the tank, put the nozzle back, close the tank, pay, turn car on, leave). Schemas make the world more predictable, allowing expectations to be formed of how things will enfold and to take note of things that happen out of context.[16] However, schemas also allow for memory errors, in that if certain aspects of a scene or event are missing from memory, people may incorrectly remember having actually seen or experienced them because they are usually a regular aspect of the schema. For example, an individual may not remember paying the waiter, but may believe they have done so, as this is a regular step in the script of going to a restaurant. Similarly, a person may recall seeing a fridge in a picture of a kitchen, even if one was not actually depicted due to existing schemas which suggest that kitchens almost always contain a fridge.[16]

Intrusion errorsEdit

Intrusion errors refer to when information that is related to the theme of a certain memory, but was not actually a part of the original episode, become associated with the event.[17] This makes it difficult to distinguish which elements are in fact part of the original memory. One idea regarding how intrusion errors work is due to a lack of recall inhibition, which allows irrelevant information to be brought to awareness while attempting to remember.[18] Another possible explanation is that intrusion errors result from a lack of new context integration into a viable memory trace, or into an already existing memory trace that is related to the appropriate memory.[18] More explanations involve the temporal aspect of recall, meaning that as the time difference between the study periods of different lists approaches zero, the amount of intrusions between the lists tends to increase,[19] the semantic aspect, meaning that the list of target words may have induced a false recall of non-target words that happen to have a similar or same meaning as the targets,[20] and the similarity aspect, for example subjects who were given list of letters to recall were likely to replace target vowels with non-target vowels.[21]

Intrusion errors can be divided into two categories. The first are known as extra-list errors, which occur when incorrect and non-related items are recalled, and were not part of the word study list.[18] These types of intrusion errors often follow the DRM Paradigm effects, in which the incorrectly recalled items are often thematically related to the study list one is attempting to recall from. Another pattern for extra-list intrusions would be an acoustic similarity pattern, this pattern states that targets that have a similar sound to non-targets may be replaced with those non-targets in recall.[22] One major type of extra-list intrusions is called the «Prior-List Intrusion» (PLI), a PLI occurs when targets from previously studied lists are recalled instead of the targets in the current list. PLIs often follow under the temporal aspect of intrusions in that since they were recalled recently they have a high chance of being recalled now.[19] The second type of intrusion error is known as intra-list errors, which is similar to extra-list errors, except it refers to irrelevant recall for items that were on the word study list.[18] Although these two categories of intrusion errors are based on word list studies in laboratories, the concepts can be extrapolated to real-life situations. Also, the same three factors that play a critical role in correct recall (recency, temporal association and semantic relatedness) play a role in intrusions as well.[19]

Time-slice errorsEdit

Time-slice errors occur when a correct event is in fact recalled; however the event that was asked to be recalled is not the one that is recalled. In other words, the timing of events is incorrectly remembered.[23] As discovered in a study by Brewer (1988), often the event or event details that are recalled occurred within a short time proximity to the memory required to be recalled.[23] There are three possible theories as to why time-slice errors occur. First, they may be a form of interference, in which the memory information from one time impairs the recall of information from a different time.[24] (see interference below). A second theory is that intrusion errors may be responsible, in that memories revolving around a similar time period thus share a common theme, and memories of various points of time within that larger time period become mixed with each other and intrude on each other’s recall. Last, the recall of memories often have holes due to forgotten details. Thus, individuals may be using a script (see schema errors) to help fill in these holes with general knowledge of what they know happened around this time. Since scripts are a time-based knowledge structure, which puts details of a memory in sequence to make it easier to understand, time-slice errors can occur if a detail is mistakenly placed in the wrong sequence.[24]

Personal life effectsEdit

Personal life effects refer to the recall and belief in events as declared by family members or friends for having actually happened.[25] Personal life effects are largely based on suggestive influences from external sources, such as family members or a therapist.[7] Other influential sources may include media or literature stories which involve details that are believed to have been experienced or witnessed, such as a natural disaster close to where one resides, or a situation that is common and could have occurred, such as getting lost as a child. Personal life effects are most powerful when claimed to be true by a family member, and even more powerful when a secondary source confirms the event having happened.[7]

Personal life effects are believed to be a form of source confusion, in which the individual cannot recall where the memory is coming from.[26] Therefore, without being able to confirm the source of the memory, the individual may accept the false memory as true. Three factors may be responsible for the implantation of false autobiographical memories. The first factor is time. As time passes, memories fade. Therefore, source confusion may result due to time delay.[7] The second factor is the imagination inflation effect. As the amount of imagination increases, so does one’s familiarity for the contents of the imagination. Thus, source confusion may also occur due to the individual confusing the source of the memory for being true, when in fact it was imaginary.[26] Lastly, social pressure to recall the memory may affect the individual’s belief in the false memory. For example, with increase in pressure, the individual may lower their criteria for validating a memory, and come to accept a false memory for being true.[26] Personal life effects can be extremely crucial to recognize in cases of recovered memories, especially those of abuse, in which the individual may have been led to believe they had been abused as a child by a therapist during psychological therapy, when in fact they had not been. Personal life effects can also be important in witness testimonies, in which suggestions from authorities may incorrectly implant memories regarding witnesses a particular detail about a crime (see the Childhood Abuse and Eye Witness Testimony sections below).

Memory error relating to foodEdit

In a study done by J. Mojet and E.P. Köster, participants were asked to have a drink, yoghurt, and some biscuits for breakfast. A few hours later, they were asked to identify and the items they had during breakfast out of five variations of the drinks, yoghurts, and biscuits. The results showed that participants remember the texture of the foods much better than for fatty content, although they could discern the difference of both among the different items. Participants were also most certain about foods that they did not have during breakfast, but were the least certain about foods that they said were in their breakfast and about foods that were in their breakfast but were not recognized. This suggests that incidental and implicit memory for foods are more focused on discerning new and potentially unsafe food rather than remembering the details of familiar foods.[27]

CausesEdit

Cognitive factorsEdit

Spreading activationEdit

One theory on how memory works is through a concept called spreading activation. Spreading activation refers to the firing of connected nodes in associated memory links.[28] The theory states that memory is organized in a theoretical web of associated ideas and related features. Each feature, or node, is connected to related features or ideas, which in turn are themselves connected to other related nodes.[29]

Spreading activation can also demonstrate how memory errors may occur. As the network of memory associations increases in the number of connections–the connection density–the likelihood of memory gaps and errors occurring also increases. Put simply, the amount of activation a secondary connection receives depends on how many connections the initial node has associated with it. This is because the initial node must divide the amount of activation it spreads to related nodes by the number of connecting nodes it is associated with. If node 1 has three connecting nodes, and node 2 has 15 connecting nodes, the three connecting nodes from node 1 will receive greater activation levels (the activation level is divided less) than for the 15 connecting nodes from node 2, and the components of these nodes will be more easily brought to mind. What this means is that, with more connections a node has, the more difficulty there is bringing to mind one of its connecting features.[30] This can lead to memory errors, in that if the connection density is so great that there is not enough activation reaching the secondary nodes, then the person may not recall a target memory that is actually present, and a memory error occurs.

Activation levels to secondary nodes is also determined by the strength of the association to the primary node.[28] Some connections have greater association with the primary node (e.g. fire truck and fire or red, versus fire truck and hose or Dalmatian), and thus will receive a greater portion of the divided activation than less associated connections. Thus, associations that receive less activation will be out-competed by associations with stronger connections, and may fail to be brought into awareness, again causing a memory error.

Connection densityEdit

The connection densities, or neighbourhood densities[31] of memory arrangements help distinguish which elements are a part of, or related to, the target memory. As the density of neural networks increases, the number of retrieval cues (associated nodes) also increases, which may allow for enhanced memory of the event.[30] However, too many connections can inhibit memory in two ways. First, as described under the sub-section Spreading Activation, the total activation being spread from node 1 to connecting nodes is divided by the number of connections. With a greater number of connections, each connecting node receives less activation, which may result in too little activation for the memory cue to be brought to awareness. Connection strength, in which more strongly connected associations receive more activation than less-related associations, may also prevent specific connections from being brought to awareness due to being out-competed by the stronger associations.[31] Second, with more connections branching from various other nodes, there is a greater probability of linking associated connections of different memories together (transplant errors) so that memory errors occur and incorrect features are recalled.

Retrieval cuesEdit

A retrieval cue is a type of hint that can be used to evoke a memory that has been stored but cannot be recalled. Retrieval cues work by selecting traces or associations in memory that contain specific content. With regards to the theory of spreading activation, retrieval cues use associated nodes to help activate a specific or target node.[32] When no cues are available, recall is greatly reduced, leading to forgetting and possible memory errors. This is called retrieval failure, or cue-dependent forgetting.

Retrieval cues can be divided into two subsets, although they are by no means used independent of each other. The first are called feature cues, in which information of the content of the original memory, or related content, is used to aid recall.[32] The second category is context cues, in which information based on the specific context (environment) in which the memory or learning occurred is used to aid recall.[32]

Although retrieval cues are usually helpful in helping to remember something, they can produce memory errors in three ways. First, incorrect cues can be used, leading to recall of an incorrect memory. Second, although the correct retrieval cues may be used, they may still produce an incorrect memory. This is likely to occur with high connection densities, in which the incorrect (but associated) node was activated and thus recalled, instead of the target memory. Third, the retrieval cue chosen may be correct and associated with the target memory, but it may not have a strong connection to the target memory, and thus may not produce enough activation to produce the target memory.

Encoding specificityEdit

Encoding specificity is when retrieval is successful to the extent that the retrieval cues used to help recall, match the cues the individual used during learning or encoding.[33] Memory errors due to encoding specificity means that the memory is likely not forgotten, however, the specific cues used during encoding the primary event are now unavailable to help remember the event. The cues used during encoding are dependent on the environment of the individual at the time the memory occurred. In context-dependent memory, recall is based on the difference between the encoding and recall environments.[34] Recall for items learned in a particular context is better when recall occurs in the same place as when the initial memory occurred. This is why it is sometimes useful to “return to the scene of the crime” to help witnesses remember details of a crime, or for explaining why going to a specific location such as a residence or community setting, may lead to becoming flooded with memories of things that happened in that context. Recall can also depend on state-dependency, or the conditions of one’s internal environment, at both the time of the event and of recall.[35] For example, if intoxicated at the time the memory actually occurred, recall for details of the event is greater when recalling while intoxicated. Associated with state-dependency, recall can also depend on mood-dependency, in which recall is greater when the mood for when the memory occurred matches the mood during recall.[23] These specific dependencies are based on the fact that the cues used during the initial event can be specific to the context or state one was in. In other words, various features of the environment (both internal and external) can be used to help encode the memory, and thus become retrieval cues. However, if the context or state changes at the time of recall, these cues may no longer be available, and thus recall is hindered.

Transfer-appropriate processingEdit

Memory errors can also depend on the method of encoding used when initially experiencing or learning information, known as transfer-appropriate processing.[36] Encoding processes can occur at three levels: visual form (the letters that make up a word), phonology (the sound of a word), and semantics (the meaning of the word or sentence). With relation to memory errors, the level of processing at the time of encoding and at the time of recall should be the same.[37] Although semantic processing generally produces greater recall that shallower levels of processing, a study by Morris et al. demonstrated that what might be the key factor to greater recall is transfer-appropriate processing–when the level of processing at the original memory/learning time matches the level of processing used to help recall. In other words, if learning occurred by rhyming the target words to other words, then recall is best if testing is also at the phonological level of processing, such as a rhyming recognition test. Thus, memory errors can occur when the levels of processing between encoding and recall do not match.[37]

InterferenceEdit

Interference occurs when specific information inhibits learning and /or recall for a specific memory.[38] There are two forms of interference. First, proactive interference has to do with difficulty in learning new material based on the inability to override information from older memories.[39] In such cases, retrieval cues continue to be associated and aimed at recalling previously learned information, affecting the recall of new material. Retroactive interference is the opposite of proactive interference, in which there is difficulty in the recall of previously learned information based on the interference of newly acquired information. In this case, retrieval cues are associated with the new information and not the older memory.[40] thus affecting recall of older material. Interference of either form can produce memory errors, in which there is interference with the recall of material. In other words, previously used retrieval cues are no longer associated with prior memories, and thus memory confusion or even an inability to recall the memory can occur.

Physiological factorsEdit

Brain damageEdit

Lobes of the human brain

Frontal lobe

Temporal lobe

Parietal lobe

Occipital
lobe

Damage in the temporal lobe (green) and frontal lobe (blue) are associated with resulting memory errors

Neuroimaging studies have provided evidence for links between brain damage and memory errors. Brain areas implicated include the frontal lobe and medial-temporal regions of the brain. Such damage may result in significant confabulations and source confusion.[8]
The prefrontal cortex is in charge of making heuristic judgments and systematic judgments, which involve analyzing the qualities of memories and the retrieval and evaluation of supporting or disconfirming information.[8] Thus, if the frontal region is damaged, these abilities will be impaired and memories may not be retrieved or evaluated correctly. For example, one patient who suffered frontal lobe damage after an automobile accident reported memories of the support his family provided for him after the accident, which in reality, was false.[41]

The temporal region of the brain contains the hippocampus, which plays a significant role in memory.[42] Thus, damage in this region may impair the function of this brain structure and result in memory problems.

AgeEdit

Studies have shown that the likelihood of memory errors occurring increases as age increases. Possible reasons for this are increased source confusion for the event and findings that older persons have decreased levels of processing when first presented with new information.[43][44] Source confusion refers to the inability to distinguish how one came upon different information. Older individuals can become confused with where they learnt information (e.g. TV, radio, newspaper, word of mouth, etc.), who presented them with the information (e.g. which of two experimenters presented them with facts and which presented them with irrelevant information), and whether the information came from an imagined source, and is thus not factual, or a real world source.[44] This in itself is a form of memory error but can also create larger memory errors. When an older individual is confused whether the information is factual or was imagined they can begin to accept imagined memories as real and thus begin to rely on new false information.[44]

Levels of processing refers to the manner in which someone encodes information into their memory and how deeply it is encoded.[44] There are three different levels of processing ranging from shallow to deep, deep being stored in long-term memory for a longer period and thus better remembered. The three levels are; visual form, being the shallowest form, phonology, being a medium level of processing, and semantics (meaning), which is the deepest form of processing.[44] The visual form of processing relies on the ability to see information and break it down into its components (e.g. see the word «dog», composed D, O, and G). Phonology relies on creating links to information through sound such as cues and tricks for memory (e.g. Dog rhymes with Fog).[44] Finally, semantics refer to the creation of meaning behind information such as adding detail to allow the information to create links throughout our memory with other memories and thus be held in long-term memory for a longer period (e.g. A dog is a four-legged pet that often chases cats and chews on bones).[44] Older individuals often begin to lose the quick ability to add meaning to new information, which leads to shallower processing and easier forgetting of the information gained.[44] Both of these possible factors can decrease the validity of the memory due to retrieval for details of the event being more difficult to perform. This leads to details of memories being confused with others or lost completely, which lead to many errors in recall of a memory.

EmotionEdit

The emotional impact of an event can have a direct impact on how the memory is first encoded, how it is later recalled, and what details of the event are accurately recalled. Highly emotional events tend to be recalled easily due to their emotional impact,[45] as well as their distinctiveness relative to other memories (highly emotional events do not occur on a regular basis, and thus are easily separated from other events that are more commonly met). Emotional events may affect memory not only because of their distinctiveness, but also because of their arousal on the individual.[46] Studies have found that prime or central features of such highly emotional events tend to be accurately recalled, whereas subtle details of the events are not remembered, or are remembered with vague consistency. Memory errors related to highly emotional events are influenced in ways such as:

  • Whether the event was positive or negative in nature — The nature of the event can affect memory, negative events tend to be remembered with greater accuracy than positive events.[23]
  • Implicit theories of consistency and change — This term was coined by Ross (1989), and is used to describe the phenomena of memory change based on the belief of how the person felt at the time of the event compared to their current feelings about it.[23] In other words, the memory of one’s emotion towards an event can change depending on their current emotional state toward the same event. If a person believes their feelings at both times continue to be the same, then the current emotion to «remember» how they felt about the event at a previous time is used. If feelings are believed to have changed, then recall of the emotional involvement in the past event is adjusted to the current feelings.
  • Intrusion errors — This term refers to the inclusion of details that may have been commonly experienced in the event, but not by the individual. For example, in the September 11 terrorist attacks, many people may state that they remember hearing about the attacks on the television news, as this was a common way of finding out this information, whereas they may have actually heard about it from a neighbour or on the radio.[47]
  • Mood-congruency — Items/events are better recalled when the mood of the individual at the time of the event and the time of recall are the same. Thus, if the mood at the time of recall does not match the mood experienced at the time the event occurred, there is an increased chance that complete recall will be affected/interrupted.[48]

Memory errors in Abnormal PsychologyEdit

Abnormal psychology is the branch of psychology that studies unusual patterns of behavior, emotion and thought, which may or may not be understood as a mental disorder. Memory errors can commonly be found in types of abnormal psychology such as Alzheimer’s disease, depression, and schizophrenia.

Alzheimer’s diseaseEdit

Alzheimer’s disease is characterized by progressive memory impairment and decline, usually beginning short-term memory.[49] As it is a progressive disease, Alzheimer’s usually begins with memory errors before proceeding to long-term memory stores. One form of memory error occurs in contrast to the theory of retrieval cues in being a reason for the occurrence of memory errors. As noted above, memory errors may be due to the lack of cues that can trigger a memory trace and bring it to awareness. However, studies have shown that the opposite may be true for patients with Alzheimer’s, such that cues may actually decrease perform on
priming tasks.[50] Patients also demonstrate errors known as misattribution errors, otherwise known as source confusion. However, studies show that these misattribution errors are dependent on whether the task is a familiarity or recollection task.[51]

Although patients tend to exhibit a higher level of false recognitions than control groups,[52] researchers have shown that they may exhibit less false-recognition early in the test due to familiarity being slower to develop. However, the observation of increasing instances of misattribution errors can be seen once familiarity does occur.[51] This may be related to the retrieval cue speculation, in that familiar memories often contain cues we know, and thus may be the reason why familiarity can contribute to memory errors. Lastly, many studies have shown that Alzheimer’s patients commonly suffer from intrusion errors. Relative to the findings that retrieval cues may actually hurt recall performance, one study by Kramer et al. showed that intrusions are most commonly associated with cue-recall tasks.[53] This study suggests that cues may lead to intrusions because patients may have a difficult time distinguishing between cues and distractions,[53] which may help explain why cues increase memory errors in patients with Alzheimer’s. Since verbal intrusions are a common aspect of Alzheimer’s,[54] some researchers believe that this characteristic may be helpful in the diagnosis of the disease.[55]

DepressionEdit

Memory errors can occur in patients with depression or with depressive symptoms. Patients with depressive symptoms have a tendency to experience what is known as the negative triad, which is the perspective use of negative schemas and self-concepts to relate to the external world. Due to this negative triad, depressive patients have a tendency to have a much greater focus on, and recall for, negative details and events over positive ones. This may lead to memory errors related to the positive details of memories, impairing the accuracy or even complete recall of such memories. Depressed patients also experience deficits in psychomotor speed and in free recall of material both immediate and delayed. This suggests that material has been encoded but that patients are particularly impaired with regard to search and retrieval processes.[56] Diverse aspects of memory such as short-term memory, long-term memory, semantic memory and implicit memory, have been studied and linked to depression. Short-term memory, a temporary store for newly acquired information, seems to show no major impairments in the case of depressive patients who do seem to complain about poor concentration, which by itself can cause simple memory errors.[56]

Long-term memory, large capacity able to retain information over long periods of time, does however show impairment in the case of depressed individuals. They tend to have difficulties in recall and recognition for both verbal and visuo-spatial material with intervals of a few minutes or even hours creating complex memory errors in relation to speech and surrounding details.[56] Individuals suffering with depression also showed a specific deficit in retrieving information meaningfully organized in their semantic memory, conceptual knowledge about the real world.[56] Therefore, depressive patients can show memory errors for the most meaningful events in their lives, unable to recall these specific moments vividly like someone not suffering from depression might. In the case of implicit memory, when previous information influences ongoing responses, there has been little to no proof of a deficit in relations to depressed individuals.[56]

SchizophreniaEdit

Memory errors, specifically intrusion errors and imagination inflation effects, have been found to occur in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia. Intrusion errors can commonly be found in the recall portion of a memory test when a participant includes items that were not on the original list that was presented.[57] These types of errors are linked to problems with self-monitoring, increased positive and disorganized symptoms (confusion within the brain), and poor executive functioning.[57] Intrusion errors are found to be more likely in patients with positive schizophrenic symptoms, which involve an excess of normal bodily functions (e.g. delusions), versus negative schizophrenic symptoms, which involve a decrease in normal bodily functions (e.g. refusal to speak).[58] Possible reasons for this are reduced function in the central executive of the working memory, as well as defects in self-reflectivity, organization and reasoning. Self-reflectivity is the ability to recognize and reason about one’s own thought process, recognize that one has thoughts, and that those thoughts are one’s own and differentiate between cognitive operations.[57] Self-reflectivity has been shown to be one of the biggest deficits faced by schizophrenics and data suggests that verbal memory intrusions are linked to deficits in the ability to identify, organize, and reason about one’s own thoughts in patients with schizophrenia.[57]

Imagination inflation effects were also common memory errors in patients with schizophrenia. This effect refers to events that individuals have imagined so vividly in their minds that this adds belief to the fact that the event truly occurred, although it did not. Possible reasons for this are increased source confusion and/or decreased source recollection of an event, which shows poor use of source-monitoring processes.[59] Source-monitoring processes allow one to distinguish between a memory that we may believe has happened because it seems familiar and one that has truly occurred. In the case of schizophrenics, whose abilities to reason through their thoughts is impaired, something that they have imagined and thus, seems familiar can easily be mistaken for an actual event, especially in the case of quick heuristic processes and snap judgments.[59] Continuously imagining an action or event makes this action more and more familiar thus making it harder for a patient with schizophrenia to distinguish its source, questioning whether it is familiar because they have imagined it or if it is familiar because it happened. This leads to many memory errors for these individuals who are led to believe by their imagination of the event that it is real, has occurred and thus is stored in their memory for that reason.[59]

Consequences of memory errorsEdit

Memory errors often lead to the belief of new memories, which are problematic. In the case of eyewitness testimony new false memories can often lead to wrong information and lack of conviction or wrongful conviction of individuals. Also in the case of child abuse, memory errors can lead to the creation of false traumatic childhood memories, which can lead to false accusations and loss of trust.

Eyewitness testimonyEdit

Memory errors can occur in eyewitness testimonies due to a number of features commonly present in a trial, all of which may influence the authenticity of the memory, and may be detrimental to the outcome of the case at hand.
Such features include:

Leading questions
refers to how wording of questions can influence how an event is remembered. This can result from a misinformation effect or an imagination inflation effect.[60] The misinformation effect occurs when information is presented after the events in question have occurred which leads to memory errors in later retrieval.[61] Studies have suggested that witnesses may misattribute accuracy to misleading information because the sources of misleading information and witnessed information become confused.[61] Misleading information can be acquired through reading of the newspaper, watching the news, being interviewed or when sitting in the courthouse during the trial. When witnesses are asked to recall specific details of an event they can begin to doubt their memory, which can cause memory errors. Misinformation can manifest itself as a memory leading the individual to believe it as true and witnesses may also begin to doubt their own memories of the event deciding instead that they must be wrong.[61] Memory errors also occur through the imagination inflation effect. As stated earlier, the imagination inflation effect takes place when an individual imagines an event to the point where it is believed as a memory of an actual event.[60] During trial, witnesses hear many different possible occurrences of events and are led to imagine these situations. Through imagining and rehearsal of the occurrences, witnesses may begin to see vividness and validity in the stories simply from rehearsal, not factual memories.[60] This can create problems for witnesses when trying to distinguish between imagined events and the actual occurrence of the events. Small but largely significant details become easily mixed and these occurrences of memory errors can make or break a trial.
Weapons Focus Effect
refers to the fact that witnesses are highly likely to pay close attention to the weapon being used during an event, which creates a reduction in the ability to remember other details regarding the crime.[62] This can in turn create memory errors leaving the witness less able to remember details such as what the assailant was wearing or what distinctive features could be found on their body or face. One explanation for why witnesses tend to gravitate toward the weapon being used is said to be that the arousal of the witness is increased.[63] When arousal becomes increased the number of perceptional cues being utilized by the brain decreases.[63] This allows the individual to focus on the weapon cue and ignore other cues such as distinct scars or a bright red shirt. The weapons focus effect can also refer to how the report of the use of weapons in the case can influence the memory of the event, leading to a false memory of having heard a weapon being fired even if the witness did not.[63] For example, if a newspaper reports that the victim was beaten with a hammer, upon reading this, the witness will begin to believe that a hammer was in fact used, even if they at no point saw a hammer. This can cause many memory errors and conflict of stories for witnesses. As a society we believe that newspapers or televised news reports have fact behind them. If they report the hammer being used, a witness might begin to second-guess their memory wondering if they missed the hammer or failed to remember that detail.[62] Also, their story may become mixed with the media’s representation of the story and the knife that they did see will be forgotten and instead be replaced with the hammer that was reported.[63]
Familiarity Effect
refers to the tendency of individuals to develop a preference for things simply because they are familiar.[64] This can leave an individual to identify familiar people as guilty, even if they are not. When we are continuously exposed to the same object or person we begin to develop a positive attraction towards said object or person. Simply their familiarity creates a positive sense when re-exposed to the individual or object. In reality one can know very little about a person but by seeing them over and over again can gain unconscious positive recall for their face. This can create memory errors when individuals are asked to identify a criminal and someone familiar to them is placed in the line up. When a familiar face is in among the individuals that the witness is being asked to study, the witness will find himself or herself gravitating towards the familiar face whether or not this is who they witnessed committing the crime. This leaves them more likely to ignore the cues that are leading them towards other individuals and concentrate on the familiar face, resulting in a false accusation. The sense of familiarity can play a large role in the identification of criminals but when the familiarity of a criminal is mixed in with the familiarity of other individuals, choosing the right person can become quite difficult.

Child abuseEdit

Memory errors regarding the recovery of repressed childhood abuse can occur due to post-event suggestions from a trusted source, such as a family member, or more commonly, a mental health professional. Due to possible relationships between childhood abuse and mental illness later in life, some mental health professionals believe in the Freudian theory of repressed memories as a defense mechanism for the anxiety that recall of the abuse would cause. Freud said that repression operates unconsciously in individuals who are not able to recall a threatening situation or may even forget that the abusive individual was ever part of their lives. Therefore, mental health professionals will sometimes seek to uncover possible instances of childhood abuse in patients, which may lead to suggestibility and cause a false memory of childhood abuse to arise, in an attempt to seek a cause to a mental illness.[65] No matter the confidence in the memory, this does not necessarily equate to the memory being true. This is an example of the misinformation effect and false memory effect. The fact that memories are not retrieved as whole entities but rather are reconstructed from information remaining in memory and other related knowledge make them easily susceptible to memory errors.[66] This explains why working with mental health professionals and leading questions can sometimes manifest false memories by creating knowledge of possible events and asking individuals to focus on if these events actually took place.[67] Individuals begin to overthink these situations visualizing them in their mind and overanalyzing them. This in turn leads to the belief of situations and vivid memories. Patients are left with memories they believe are real and new events from their childhood which can lead to stress and trauma in their adult life and loss of relationships with those who are believed to be the abuser.

See alsoEdit

  • Amnesia
  • False memory syndrome
  • Memory implantation
  • Memory loss
  • Memory and aging
  • Memory bias
  • Memory conformity
  • Kleptomnesia

ReferencesEdit

  1. ^ a b Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814
  2. ^ a b c d e f Schacter, Daniel L. (2011). Psychology Second Edition. New York: Worth Publishers. pp. 246. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
  3. ^ a b c d Schacter, Daniel (2011). Psychology 2nd Ed. New York: Worth Publishers. p. 243.
  4. ^ Schacter, Daniel L.,[«Psychology 2nd Ed.»],»Worth Publishers»,2009,2011.
  5. ^ a b Schacter, Daniel L., Daniel T. Gilbert, and Daniel M. Wegner. «Chapter 6: Memory.» Psychology. ; Second Edition. N.p.: Worth, Incorporated, 2011. 244-45.
  6. ^ Schacter, Daniel L., Daniel T. Gilbert, and Daniel M. Wegner. «Chapter 6: Memory.» Psychology. ; Second Edition. N.p.: Worth, Incorporated, 2011. 245.
  7. ^ a b c d e f Loftus, E. (1997). Creating false memories. Scientific American, 277, 70–75
  8. ^ a b c Johnson, M. & Raye, C. (1998). False memories and confabulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(4), 137–145
  9. ^ a b c d e f g Schacter, Daniel L. (2011). Psychology Second Edition. New York: Worth Publishers. pp. 253–254. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
  10. ^ Loftus, E. F. & Hoffman, H. G. (1989). Misinformation and memory, the creation of new memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 118(1), 100–104
  11. ^ Loftus, E. F. & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585–589
  12. ^ a b Deffenbacher, K.A., Bornstein, B.H., & Penrod, S.D. (2006). Mugshot exposure effects: Retroactive interference, mugshot commitment, source confusion, and unconscious transference. Law and Human Behaviour, 30(3), 287–307
  13. ^ Daniel L. Schacter, «Searching for memory: the brain, the mind, and the past», (New York, 1996)
  14. ^ Mazzoni, G. & Memon, A. (2003). Imagination can create false autobiographical memories. Psychological Science, 14(2), 186–188
  15. ^ Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22
  16. ^ a b c d Kleider, H.M., Pezdek, K., Goldinger, S.D., & Kirk, A. (2008). Schema-driven source misattribution errors: Remembering the expected from a witnessed event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1–20
  17. ^ Jacobs, D. (1990). Intrusion errors in the figural memory of patients with Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, 49–57.
  18. ^ a b c d Stip, E., Corbière, M., Boulay, L. J., Lesage, A., Lecomte, T., Leclerc, C., Richard, N., Cyr, M., & Guillem, F. (2007). Intrusion errors in explicit memory: Their differential relationship with clinical and social outcome in chronic schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 12(2), 112–127
  19. ^ a b c Kahana et al. (2006). Temporal Associations and Prior list intrusions in Free Recall.
  20. ^ Smith, Troy, A., Kimball, Daniel, R. Kahana, Michael, J. (2007). The fSAM Model of False Recall.
  21. ^ Wickelgren, Wayne, A., (1965. Similarity and Intrusions in Short Term Memory for Consonant-Vowel Digrams.
  22. ^ Wickelgren, Wayne, A., (1965). Acoustic Similarity and Intrusion Errors in Short Term Memory.
  23. ^ a b c d e Hyman Jr., I.E., & Loftus, E.F. (1998). Errors in autobiographical memory. Clinical Psychology Review, 18(8), 933–947
  24. ^ a b Greenberg, D.L. (2004). President Bush’s false «flashbulb» memory of 9/11/01. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 363–370
  25. ^ Laney, C., & Loftus, E.F. (2010). Truth in emotional memories. In B.H. Bornstein (Ed.), Emotion and the Law (pp. 157–183). Leicester: Springer Science + Business Media
  26. ^ a b c Wade, K.A., & Garry, M. (2005). Strategies for verifying false autobiographical memories. American Journal of Psychology, 118(4), 587–602
  27. ^ Mojet J, Ko¨ster EP. Sensory memory and food texture. Food Qual Prefer 2005; 16: 251–266. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2004.04.017.
  28. ^ a b Dell, G.S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93(3), 283–321
  29. ^ Anderson, J.R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 261–295
  30. ^ a b Kapatsinski, V. (2004). Frequency, age-of-acquisition, lexicon size, neighborhood density, and speed of processing: towards a domain-general, single mechanism account. High Desert Linguistic Society Conference, 6, 131–150.
  31. ^ a b Forster, K.I. (1991). The density constraint on form-priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(1), 1–25.
  32. ^ a b c Jaimes, A., Omura, K., Nagamine, T., & Hirata, K. (2004). Memory cues for meeting video retrieval. In
    Proceedings of the 1st ACM Workshop on Continuous Archival and Retrieval of Personal Experiences
    (CARPE) at the ACM International Multimedia Conference
  33. ^ Tulving, E., & Thomson, D.M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352–373
  34. ^ Smith, S.M. (1984). A comparison of two techniques for reducing context-dependent forgetting. Memory and Cognition, 12(5), 477–482.
  35. ^ Eich, J.E. (1980). The cue-dependent nature of state-dependent retrieval. Memory and Cognition, 8(2), 157–173.
  36. ^ Thomson, D.M., & Tulving, E. (1970). Associative encoding and retrieval: Weak and strong cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86(2), 255–262.
  37. ^ a b Moscovitc, M., & Craik, F.I.M. (1976). Depth of processing, retrieval cues, and uniqueness of encoding as factors in recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 447–458.
  38. ^ Baddeley, A.D., & Dale, H.C.A. (1966). The effect of semantic similarity on retro-active interference in long- and short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 417–420.
  39. ^ Badre, D. & Wagner, A.D. (2005). Frontal lobe mechanisms that resolve proactive interference. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 2003–2012.
  40. ^ Dewar, M.T., Cowan, N., & Della Sala, S. (2007). Forgetting due to retroactive interference: A fusion of Muller and Pilzecker’s (1900) early insights into everyday forgetting and recent research on anterograde amnesia. Cortex, 43(5), 616–634.
  41. ^ Conway, M. A., & Tacchi, P. C. (1996). Motivated confabulation. Neurocase, 2, 325–338
  42. ^ Treves, A. & Rolls, E. T. (1994). Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory. Neuroscience, 4(3), 374–391
  43. ^ Kensinger, E.A., & Corkin, S. (2004). The effects of emotional content and aging on false memories. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioural Neuroscience, 4(1), 1–9.
  44. ^ a b c d e f g h Schacter, D.L., Koutstaal, W., & Norman, K.A. (1997). False memories and aging. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(6).
  45. ^ Kensinger, E.A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 1(2), 99–113
  46. ^ Dolcos, F., & Denkova, E. (2008). Neural correlates of encoding emotional memories: A review of functional neuroimaging evidence. Cell Science, 5(2)
  47. ^ Schmidt, S.R. (2004). Autobiographical memories for the September 11 attacks: Reconstructive errors and emotional impairment of memory. Memory and Cognition, 32(3), 443–454
  48. ^ Ruci, L., Tomes, J.L., & Zelenski, J.M. (2009). Mood-congruent false memories in the DRM paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 23(6), 1153–1165
  49. ^ Baddeley, A.D., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., Logie, R., & Spinnler, H. (1991). The decline of working memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 114, 2521–2542.
  50. ^ Mimura, M., & Komatsu, S.I. (2010). Factors of error and effort in memory intervention for patients with Alzheimer’s disease and amnesic syndrome. Psychogeriatrics, 10, 179–186.
  51. ^ a b Mitchell, J.P., Sullivan, A.L., Schacter, D.L., & Budson, A.E. (2006). Misattribution errors in Alzheimer’s disease: The illusory truth effect. Neuropsychology, 20(2), 185–192.
  52. ^ Hildebrandt, H., Haldenwanger, A., & Eling, P. (2009). False recognition helps to distinguish patients with Alzheimer’s disease and amnesic mci from patients with other kinds of dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 28(2).
  53. ^ a b Kramer, J.H., Delis, D.C., Blusewicz, M.J., & Brandt, J. (1988). Verbal memory errors in Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s dementias. Developmental Neuropsychology, 4(1), 1–15.
  54. ^ Kern, B.S., Gorp, W.G.V., Cummings, J.L., Brown, W.S., & Osato, S.S. (1992). Confabulation in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 19(2), 172–182.
  55. ^ Fuld, P.A., Katzman, R., Davies, P., & Terry, R.D. (2004). Intrusions as a sign of Alzheimer dementia chemical and pathological verification. Annals of Neurology, 11(2), 155–159.
  56. ^ a b c d e Ilsley, J.E., Moffoot, A.P.R., & O’Carroll, R.E. (1995). An analysis of memory dysfunction in major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 35, 1–9.
  57. ^ a b c d Fridberg, D.J., Brenner, A., & Lysaker, P.H. (2010). Verbal memory intrusions in schizophrenia: Associations with self-reflectivity, symptomatology, and neurocognition. Psychiatry Research, 179, 6–11.
  58. ^ Brébion, G., Amador, X., Smith, M.J., Malaspina, D., Sharif, Z. & Gorman, J.M. (1999). Opposite links of positive and negative symptomatology with memory errors in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 88, 15–24.
  59. ^ a b c Mammarella, N., Altamura M., Padalino F.A., Petito A., Fairfield B. & Bellomo A. (2010). False memories in schizophrenia? An imagination inflation study. Psychiatry Research, 179, 267–273.
  60. ^ a b c Whitehouse, W.G., Orne, E.C., Dinges, D.F. (2010). Extreme cognitive interviewing: A blueprint for false memories through imagination inflation. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 58(3), 269–287
  61. ^ a b c English, S.M., Nielson, K.A. (2010). Reduction of the misinformation effect by arousal induced after learning. Cognition, 117(2), 237–242.
  62. ^ a b Loftus, E.F., Loftus, G.R., Messo, J. (1987). Some fact about “weapon focus”. Law and Human Behavior, 11(1), 55–62.
  63. ^ a b c d Mitchell, K.J., Livosky, M., Mather, M. (1998). The weapon focus effect revisited: The role of novelty. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 287–303
  64. ^ Moreland, R.L., Zajonc, R.B. (1982). Exposure effects in person perception: Familiarity, similarity, and attraction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 18(5), 395–415
  65. ^ Lindsay, D.S., & Read, J.D. (1994). Psychotherapy and memories of childhood sexual abuse: A cognitive perspective. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 281–338.
  66. ^ Hyman Jr., I.E., & Pentland, J. (1996). The role of mental imagery in the creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 101–117.
  67. ^ Lindsay, D.S., & Read, J.D. (1995). Memory work and recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse: Scientific evidence and public, professional, and personal issues. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 1(4), 846–908

Memory gaps and errors refer to the incorrect recall, or complete loss, of information in the memory system for a specific detail and/or event. Memory errors may include remembering events that never occurred, or remembering them differently from the way they actually happened.[1] These errors or gaps can occur due to a number of different reasons, including the emotional involvement in the situation, expectations and environmental changes. As the retention interval between encoding and retrieval of the memory lengthens, there is an increase in both the amount that is forgotten, and the likelihood of a memory error occurring.

Overview[edit]

There are several different types of memory errors, in which people may inaccurately recall details of events that did not occur, or they may simply misattribute the source of a memory. In other instances, imagination of a certain event can create confidence that such an event actually occurred. Causes of such memory errors may be due to certain cognitive factors, such as spreading activation, or to physiological factors, including brain damage, age or emotional factors. Furthermore, memory errors have been reported in individuals with schizophrenia and depression. The consequences of memory errors can have significant implications. Two main areas of concern regarding memory errors are in eyewitness testimony and cases of child abuse.

Types of memory errors[edit]

Blocking[edit]

The feeling that a person gets when they know the information, but can not remember a specific detail, like an individual’s name or the name of a place is described as the «tip-of-the-tongue» experience. The tip-of-the-tongue experience is a classic example of blocking, which is a failure to retrieve information that is available in memory even though you are trying to produce it.[2] The information you are trying to remember has been encoded and stored, and a cue is available that would usually trigger its recollection.[2] The information has not faded from memory and a person is not forgetting to retrieve the information.[2] What a person is experiencing is a complete retrieval failure, which makes blocking especially frustrating.[2] Blocking occurs especially often for the names of people and places, because their links to related concepts and knowledge are weaker than for common names.[2] The experience of blocking occurs more often as we get older; this «tip of the tongue» experience is a common complaint amongst 60- and 70-year-olds.[2]

Transience[edit]

Transience refers to forgetting what occurs with the passage of time.[3] Transience occurs during the storage phase of memory, after an experience has been encoded and before it is retrieved.[3] As time passes, the quality of our memory also changes, deteriorating from specific to more general.[3] German philosopher named Hermann Ebbinghaus decided to measure his own memory for lists of nonsense syllables at various times after studying them. He decided to draw out a curve of his forgetting pattern over time. He realized that there is a rapid drop-off in retention during the first tests and there is a slower rate of forgetting later on.[3] Therefore, transience denotes the gradual change of a specific knowledge or idea into more general memories.[4]

Absentmindedness[edit]

Absentmindedness is a gap in attention which causes memory failure. In this situation the information does not disappear from memory, it can later be recalled. But the lack of attention at a specific moment prevents the information from being recalled at that specific moment. A common cause of absentmindedness is a lack of attention.[clarification needed] Attention is vital to encoding information in long-term memory. Without proper attention, material is much less likely to be stored properly and recalled later.[5] When attention is divided, less activity in the lower left frontal lobe diminishes the ability for elaborative memory encoding to take place, and results in absentminded forgetting. More recent research has shown that divided attention also leads to less hippocampal involvement in encoding.[5] A common example of absentmindedness is not remembering to carry out actions that had been planned to be done in the future, for example, picking up grocery items, and remembering times of meetings.[6]

False memories[edit]

False memories, sometimes referred to as confabulation, refer to the recollection of inaccurate details of an event, or recollection of a whole event that never occurred. Studies investigating this memory error have been able to successfully implant memories among participants that never existed, such as being lost in a mall as a child (termed the lost in the mall technique) or spilling a bowl of punch at a wedding reception.[7] In this case, false memories were implanted among participants by their family members who claimed that the event had happened. This evidence demonstrates the possibility of implanting false memories on individuals by leading them to remember such events that never occurred. This memory error can be particularly worrisome in judicial settings, in which witnesses may have false recollections of a crime after it has happened, especially when told by others that particular things may have happened which did not.[8] Another area of concern regarding false memories is in cases of child abuse.

Problem of bias[edit]

The problem of bias, which is the distorting influences of present knowledge, beliefs, and feelings on recollection of previous experiences.[9] Sometimes what people remember from their past says less about what actually happened than about what they personally believe, feel, and the knowledge they have acquired at the present time.[9] An individual’s current moods can bias their memory recall, researchers have found.[9] There are three types of memory biases, consistency bias, change bias and egocentric bias.[9] Consistency bias is the bias to reconstruct the past to fit the present.[9] Change bias is the tendency to exaggerate differences between what we feel or believe in the present and what we previously felt or believed in the past.[9] Egocentric bias is a form of change bias, the tendency to exaggerate the change between the past and the present in order to make ourselves look good in any given situation.[9]

Misinformation effect[edit]

The misinformation effect refers to the change in memory due to the presentation of information that is relevant to the target memory, such as leading questions or suggestions.[10] Memories are likely to be altered when questions are worded differently or when inaccurate information is presented. For example, in one experiment participants watched a video of an automobile accident and were then asked questions regarding the accident. When asked how fast the automobiles were driving when they smashed into each other, the speed estimate was higher than when asked how fast the automobiles were driving when they hit, bumped or collided into each other. Similarly, participants were more likely to report there being shattered glass present when the word smashed was used instead of other verbs.[11] Evidently, memory recollection can be altered with misleading information after the event.

Source confusion[edit]

Source confusion or unconscious transference,[12] involves the misattribution of the source of a memory. For instance, an individual may recall seeing an event in person when in reality they only witnessed the event on television.[12] Ultimately, the individual has an inability to remember the source of information in which the content and the source become dissociated. This may be more likely for more distant memories, such as childhood memories.[7] In more severe cases of source confusion, you can take fictional stories you heard from when you were younger and assimilated these stories being your childhood. For example, say your father told you stories about his life when he was a child every night before you went to sleep when you were a child. When you grow up, you might mistakenly remember these stories your dad told you as your own and integrate them into your childhood memories.[13]

Imagination inflation[edit]

Imagination inflation refers to when a person remembers details of a memory that are exaggerated versions of the actual event or remember an entire memory that never occurred due to the act of imagination.[14] That is, when one imagines an event occurring, their confidence that this event actually did occur increases. One reason for this may be due to the act of imagination increasing the familiarity of the event. When the event seems more familiar, it may become more likely for people to report it actually occurring. For instance, in an experiment participants were asked to imagine playing inside and then running outside toward a window, falling and breaking it, while other participants did not imagine anything. Participants who had imagined this scenario reported an increased level of confidence that the event had actually happened in comparison to those who did not imagine the event.[7] This error can be caused simply by imagining an event.

Deese–Roediger–McDermott (DRM) paradigm[edit]

Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm refers to the incorrect recall of features of an event that were not actually present, due to the features being related to a common theme.[15] This paradigm has been demonstrated with the use of word lists and subsequent recognition tests. For example, experiments have shown that if a research participant is presented with the words: bed rest awake tired dream wake snooze snore nap yawn drowsy, there is a high likelihood that the participant will falsely recall that the word sleep was in the list of words. These results show a significant illusion in memory, in which people remember items that were never presented simply due to their relation with other items in a common theme.[1]

Schematic errors[edit]

Schematic errors refer to the use of a schema to help reconstruct parts of an experience that cannot be remembered. This may include parts of the schema that did not actually take place, or aspects of a schema that are stereotypical of an event.[16] Schemas can be described as mental guidelines (scripts) for events that are encountered in daily life.[16] For example, when going to the gas station, there is a general pattern of how things will occur (i.e. turn car off, get out of car, open gas tank, punch the gas button, put nozzle into the tank, fill up the tank, put the nozzle back, close the tank, pay, turn car on, leave). Schemas make the world more predictable, allowing expectations to be formed of how things will enfold and to take note of things that happen out of context.[16] However, schemas also allow for memory errors, in that if certain aspects of a scene or event are missing from memory, people may incorrectly remember having actually seen or experienced them because they are usually a regular aspect of the schema. For example, an individual may not remember paying the waiter, but may believe they have done so, as this is a regular step in the script of going to a restaurant. Similarly, a person may recall seeing a fridge in a picture of a kitchen, even if one was not actually depicted due to existing schemas which suggest that kitchens almost always contain a fridge.[16]

Intrusion errors[edit]

Intrusion errors refer to when information that is related to the theme of a certain memory, but was not actually a part of the original episode, become associated with the event.[17] This makes it difficult to distinguish which elements are in fact part of the original memory. One idea regarding how intrusion errors work is due to a lack of recall inhibition, which allows irrelevant information to be brought to awareness while attempting to remember.[18] Another possible explanation is that intrusion errors result from a lack of new context integration into a viable memory trace, or into an already existing memory trace that is related to the appropriate memory.[18] More explanations involve the temporal aspect of recall, meaning that as the time difference between the study periods of different lists approaches zero, the amount of intrusions between the lists tends to increase,[19] the semantic aspect, meaning that the list of target words may have induced a false recall of non-target words that happen to have a similar or same meaning as the targets,[20] and the similarity aspect, for example subjects who were given list of letters to recall were likely to replace target vowels with non-target vowels.[21]

Intrusion errors can be divided into two categories. The first are known as extra-list errors, which occur when incorrect and non-related items are recalled, and were not part of the word study list.[18] These types of intrusion errors often follow the DRM Paradigm effects, in which the incorrectly recalled items are often thematically related to the study list one is attempting to recall from. Another pattern for extra-list intrusions would be an acoustic similarity pattern, this pattern states that targets that have a similar sound to non-targets may be replaced with those non-targets in recall.[22] One major type of extra-list intrusions is called the «Prior-List Intrusion» (PLI), a PLI occurs when targets from previously studied lists are recalled instead of the targets in the current list. PLIs often follow under the temporal aspect of intrusions in that since they were recalled recently they have a high chance of being recalled now.[19] The second type of intrusion error is known as intra-list errors, which is similar to extra-list errors, except it refers to irrelevant recall for items that were on the word study list.[18] Although these two categories of intrusion errors are based on word list studies in laboratories, the concepts can be extrapolated to real-life situations. Also, the same three factors that play a critical role in correct recall (recency, temporal association and semantic relatedness) play a role in intrusions as well.[19]

Time-slice errors[edit]

Time-slice errors occur when a correct event is in fact recalled; however the event that was asked to be recalled is not the one that is recalled. In other words, the timing of events is incorrectly remembered.[23] As discovered in a study by Brewer (1988), often the event or event details that are recalled occurred within a short time proximity to the memory required to be recalled.[23] There are three possible theories as to why time-slice errors occur. First, they may be a form of interference, in which the memory information from one time impairs the recall of information from a different time.[24] (see interference below). A second theory is that intrusion errors may be responsible, in that memories revolving around a similar time period thus share a common theme, and memories of various points of time within that larger time period become mixed with each other and intrude on each other’s recall. Last, the recall of memories often have holes due to forgotten details. Thus, individuals may be using a script (see schema errors) to help fill in these holes with general knowledge of what they know happened around this time. Since scripts are a time-based knowledge structure, which puts details of a memory in sequence to make it easier to understand, time-slice errors can occur if a detail is mistakenly placed in the wrong sequence.[24]

Personal life effects[edit]

Personal life effects refer to the recall and belief in events as declared by family members or friends for having actually happened.[25] Personal life effects are largely based on suggestive influences from external sources, such as family members or a therapist.[7] Other influential sources may include media or literature stories which involve details that are believed to have been experienced or witnessed, such as a natural disaster close to where one resides, or a situation that is common and could have occurred, such as getting lost as a child. Personal life effects are most powerful when claimed to be true by a family member, and even more powerful when a secondary source confirms the event having happened.[7]

Personal life effects are believed to be a form of source confusion, in which the individual cannot recall where the memory is coming from.[26] Therefore, without being able to confirm the source of the memory, the individual may accept the false memory as true. Three factors may be responsible for the implantation of false autobiographical memories. The first factor is time. As time passes, memories fade. Therefore, source confusion may result due to time delay.[7] The second factor is the imagination inflation effect. As the amount of imagination increases, so does one’s familiarity for the contents of the imagination. Thus, source confusion may also occur due to the individual confusing the source of the memory for being true, when in fact it was imaginary.[26] Lastly, social pressure to recall the memory may affect the individual’s belief in the false memory. For example, with increase in pressure, the individual may lower their criteria for validating a memory, and come to accept a false memory for being true.[26] Personal life effects can be extremely crucial to recognize in cases of recovered memories, especially those of abuse, in which the individual may have been led to believe they had been abused as a child by a therapist during psychological therapy, when in fact they had not been. Personal life effects can also be important in witness testimonies, in which suggestions from authorities may incorrectly implant memories regarding witnesses a particular detail about a crime (see the Childhood Abuse and Eye Witness Testimony sections below).

Memory error relating to food[edit]

In a study done by J. Mojet and E.P. Köster, participants were asked to have a drink, yoghurt, and some biscuits for breakfast. A few hours later, they were asked to identify and the items they had during breakfast out of five variations of the drinks, yoghurts, and biscuits. The results showed that participants remember the texture of the foods much better than for fatty content, although they could discern the difference of both among the different items. Participants were also most certain about foods that they did not have during breakfast, but were the least certain about foods that they said were in their breakfast and about foods that were in their breakfast but were not recognized. This suggests that incidental and implicit memory for foods are more focused on discerning new and potentially unsafe food rather than remembering the details of familiar foods.[27]

Causes[edit]

Cognitive factors[edit]

Spreading activation[edit]

One theory on how memory works is through a concept called spreading activation. Spreading activation refers to the firing of connected nodes in associated memory links.[28] The theory states that memory is organized in a theoretical web of associated ideas and related features. Each feature, or node, is connected to related features or ideas, which in turn are themselves connected to other related nodes.[29]

Spreading activation can also demonstrate how memory errors may occur. As the network of memory associations increases in the number of connections–the connection density–the likelihood of memory gaps and errors occurring also increases. Put simply, the amount of activation a secondary connection receives depends on how many connections the initial node has associated with it. This is because the initial node must divide the amount of activation it spreads to related nodes by the number of connecting nodes it is associated with. If node 1 has three connecting nodes, and node 2 has 15 connecting nodes, the three connecting nodes from node 1 will receive greater activation levels (the activation level is divided less) than for the 15 connecting nodes from node 2, and the components of these nodes will be more easily brought to mind. What this means is that, with more connections a node has, the more difficulty there is bringing to mind one of its connecting features.[30] This can lead to memory errors, in that if the connection density is so great that there is not enough activation reaching the secondary nodes, then the person may not recall a target memory that is actually present, and a memory error occurs.

Activation levels to secondary nodes is also determined by the strength of the association to the primary node.[28] Some connections have greater association with the primary node (e.g. fire truck and fire or red, versus fire truck and hose or Dalmatian), and thus will receive a greater portion of the divided activation than less associated connections. Thus, associations that receive less activation will be out-competed by associations with stronger connections, and may fail to be brought into awareness, again causing a memory error.

Connection density[edit]

The connection densities, or neighbourhood densities[31] of memory arrangements help distinguish which elements are a part of, or related to, the target memory. As the density of neural networks increases, the number of retrieval cues (associated nodes) also increases, which may allow for enhanced memory of the event.[30] However, too many connections can inhibit memory in two ways. First, as described under the sub-section Spreading Activation, the total activation being spread from node 1 to connecting nodes is divided by the number of connections. With a greater number of connections, each connecting node receives less activation, which may result in too little activation for the memory cue to be brought to awareness. Connection strength, in which more strongly connected associations receive more activation than less-related associations, may also prevent specific connections from being brought to awareness due to being out-competed by the stronger associations.[31] Second, with more connections branching from various other nodes, there is a greater probability of linking associated connections of different memories together (transplant errors) so that memory errors occur and incorrect features are recalled.

Retrieval cues[edit]

A retrieval cue is a type of hint that can be used to evoke a memory that has been stored but cannot be recalled. Retrieval cues work by selecting traces or associations in memory that contain specific content. With regards to the theory of spreading activation, retrieval cues use associated nodes to help activate a specific or target node.[32] When no cues are available, recall is greatly reduced, leading to forgetting and possible memory errors. This is called retrieval failure, or cue-dependent forgetting.

Retrieval cues can be divided into two subsets, although they are by no means used independent of each other. The first are called feature cues, in which information of the content of the original memory, or related content, is used to aid recall.[32] The second category is context cues, in which information based on the specific context (environment) in which the memory or learning occurred is used to aid recall.[32]

Although retrieval cues are usually helpful in helping to remember something, they can produce memory errors in three ways. First, incorrect cues can be used, leading to recall of an incorrect memory. Second, although the correct retrieval cues may be used, they may still produce an incorrect memory. This is likely to occur with high connection densities, in which the incorrect (but associated) node was activated and thus recalled, instead of the target memory. Third, the retrieval cue chosen may be correct and associated with the target memory, but it may not have a strong connection to the target memory, and thus may not produce enough activation to produce the target memory.

Encoding specificity[edit]

Encoding specificity is when retrieval is successful to the extent that the retrieval cues used to help recall, match the cues the individual used during learning or encoding.[33] Memory errors due to encoding specificity means that the memory is likely not forgotten, however, the specific cues used during encoding the primary event are now unavailable to help remember the event. The cues used during encoding are dependent on the environment of the individual at the time the memory occurred. In context-dependent memory, recall is based on the difference between the encoding and recall environments.[34] Recall for items learned in a particular context is better when recall occurs in the same place as when the initial memory occurred. This is why it is sometimes useful to “return to the scene of the crime” to help witnesses remember details of a crime, or for explaining why going to a specific location such as a residence or community setting, may lead to becoming flooded with memories of things that happened in that context. Recall can also depend on state-dependency, or the conditions of one’s internal environment, at both the time of the event and of recall.[35] For example, if intoxicated at the time the memory actually occurred, recall for details of the event is greater when recalling while intoxicated. Associated with state-dependency, recall can also depend on mood-dependency, in which recall is greater when the mood for when the memory occurred matches the mood during recall.[23] These specific dependencies are based on the fact that the cues used during the initial event can be specific to the context or state one was in. In other words, various features of the environment (both internal and external) can be used to help encode the memory, and thus become retrieval cues. However, if the context or state changes at the time of recall, these cues may no longer be available, and thus recall is hindered.

Transfer-appropriate processing[edit]

Memory errors can also depend on the method of encoding used when initially experiencing or learning information, known as transfer-appropriate processing.[36] Encoding processes can occur at three levels: visual form (the letters that make up a word), phonology (the sound of a word), and semantics (the meaning of the word or sentence). With relation to memory errors, the level of processing at the time of encoding and at the time of recall should be the same.[37] Although semantic processing generally produces greater recall that shallower levels of processing, a study by Morris et al. demonstrated that what might be the key factor to greater recall is transfer-appropriate processing–when the level of processing at the original memory/learning time matches the level of processing used to help recall. In other words, if learning occurred by rhyming the target words to other words, then recall is best if testing is also at the phonological level of processing, such as a rhyming recognition test. Thus, memory errors can occur when the levels of processing between encoding and recall do not match.[37]

Interference[edit]

Interference occurs when specific information inhibits learning and /or recall for a specific memory.[38] There are two forms of interference. First, proactive interference has to do with difficulty in learning new material based on the inability to override information from older memories.[39] In such cases, retrieval cues continue to be associated and aimed at recalling previously learned information, affecting the recall of new material. Retroactive interference is the opposite of proactive interference, in which there is difficulty in the recall of previously learned information based on the interference of newly acquired information. In this case, retrieval cues are associated with the new information and not the older memory.[40] thus affecting recall of older material. Interference of either form can produce memory errors, in which there is interference with the recall of material. In other words, previously used retrieval cues are no longer associated with prior memories, and thus memory confusion or even an inability to recall the memory can occur.

Physiological factors[edit]

Brain damage[edit]

Lobes of the human brain

Frontal lobe

Temporal lobe

Parietal lobe

Occipital
lobe

Damage in the temporal lobe (green) and frontal lobe (blue) are associated with resulting memory errors

Neuroimaging studies have provided evidence for links between brain damage and memory errors. Brain areas implicated include the frontal lobe and medial-temporal regions of the brain. Such damage may result in significant confabulations and source confusion.[8]
The prefrontal cortex is in charge of making heuristic judgments and systematic judgments, which involve analyzing the qualities of memories and the retrieval and evaluation of supporting or disconfirming information.[8] Thus, if the frontal region is damaged, these abilities will be impaired and memories may not be retrieved or evaluated correctly. For example, one patient who suffered frontal lobe damage after an automobile accident reported memories of the support his family provided for him after the accident, which in reality, was false.[41]

The temporal region of the brain contains the hippocampus, which plays a significant role in memory.[42] Thus, damage in this region may impair the function of this brain structure and result in memory problems.

Age[edit]

Studies have shown that the likelihood of memory errors occurring increases as age increases. Possible reasons for this are increased source confusion for the event and findings that older persons have decreased levels of processing when first presented with new information.[43][44] Source confusion refers to the inability to distinguish how one came upon different information. Older individuals can become confused with where they learnt information (e.g. TV, radio, newspaper, word of mouth, etc.), who presented them with the information (e.g. which of two experimenters presented them with facts and which presented them with irrelevant information), and whether the information came from an imagined source, and is thus not factual, or a real world source.[44] This in itself is a form of memory error but can also create larger memory errors. When an older individual is confused whether the information is factual or was imagined they can begin to accept imagined memories as real and thus begin to rely on new false information.[44]

Levels of processing refers to the manner in which someone encodes information into their memory and how deeply it is encoded.[44] There are three different levels of processing ranging from shallow to deep, deep being stored in long-term memory for a longer period and thus better remembered. The three levels are; visual form, being the shallowest form, phonology, being a medium level of processing, and semantics (meaning), which is the deepest form of processing.[44] The visual form of processing relies on the ability to see information and break it down into its components (e.g. see the word «dog», composed D, O, and G). Phonology relies on creating links to information through sound such as cues and tricks for memory (e.g. Dog rhymes with Fog).[44] Finally, semantics refer to the creation of meaning behind information such as adding detail to allow the information to create links throughout our memory with other memories and thus be held in long-term memory for a longer period (e.g. A dog is a four-legged pet that often chases cats and chews on bones).[44] Older individuals often begin to lose the quick ability to add meaning to new information, which leads to shallower processing and easier forgetting of the information gained.[44] Both of these possible factors can decrease the validity of the memory due to retrieval for details of the event being more difficult to perform. This leads to details of memories being confused with others or lost completely, which lead to many errors in recall of a memory.

Emotion[edit]

The emotional impact of an event can have a direct impact on how the memory is first encoded, how it is later recalled, and what details of the event are accurately recalled. Highly emotional events tend to be recalled easily due to their emotional impact,[45] as well as their distinctiveness relative to other memories (highly emotional events do not occur on a regular basis, and thus are easily separated from other events that are more commonly met). Emotional events may affect memory not only because of their distinctiveness, but also because of their arousal on the individual.[46] Studies have found that prime or central features of such highly emotional events tend to be accurately recalled, whereas subtle details of the events are not remembered, or are remembered with vague consistency. Memory errors related to highly emotional events are influenced in ways such as:

  • Whether the event was positive or negative in nature — The nature of the event can affect memory, negative events tend to be remembered with greater accuracy than positive events.[23]
  • Implicit theories of consistency and change — This term was coined by Ross (1989), and is used to describe the phenomena of memory change based on the belief of how the person felt at the time of the event compared to their current feelings about it.[23] In other words, the memory of one’s emotion towards an event can change depending on their current emotional state toward the same event. If a person believes their feelings at both times continue to be the same, then the current emotion to «remember» how they felt about the event at a previous time is used. If feelings are believed to have changed, then recall of the emotional involvement in the past event is adjusted to the current feelings.
  • Intrusion errors — This term refers to the inclusion of details that may have been commonly experienced in the event, but not by the individual. For example, in the September 11 terrorist attacks, many people may state that they remember hearing about the attacks on the television news, as this was a common way of finding out this information, whereas they may have actually heard about it from a neighbour or on the radio.[47]
  • Mood-congruency — Items/events are better recalled when the mood of the individual at the time of the event and the time of recall are the same. Thus, if the mood at the time of recall does not match the mood experienced at the time the event occurred, there is an increased chance that complete recall will be affected/interrupted.[48]

Memory errors in Abnormal Psychology[edit]

Abnormal psychology is the branch of psychology that studies unusual patterns of behavior, emotion and thought, which may or may not be understood as a mental disorder. Memory errors can commonly be found in types of abnormal psychology such as Alzheimer’s disease, depression, and schizophrenia.

Alzheimer’s disease[edit]

Alzheimer’s disease is characterized by progressive memory impairment and decline, usually beginning short-term memory.[49] As it is a progressive disease, Alzheimer’s usually begins with memory errors before proceeding to long-term memory stores. One form of memory error occurs in contrast to the theory of retrieval cues in being a reason for the occurrence of memory errors. As noted above, memory errors may be due to the lack of cues that can trigger a memory trace and bring it to awareness. However, studies have shown that the opposite may be true for patients with Alzheimer’s, such that cues may actually decrease perform on
priming tasks.[50] Patients also demonstrate errors known as misattribution errors, otherwise known as source confusion. However, studies show that these misattribution errors are dependent on whether the task is a familiarity or recollection task.[51]

Although patients tend to exhibit a higher level of false recognitions than control groups,[52] researchers have shown that they may exhibit less false-recognition early in the test due to familiarity being slower to develop. However, the observation of increasing instances of misattribution errors can be seen once familiarity does occur.[51] This may be related to the retrieval cue speculation, in that familiar memories often contain cues we know, and thus may be the reason why familiarity can contribute to memory errors. Lastly, many studies have shown that Alzheimer’s patients commonly suffer from intrusion errors. Relative to the findings that retrieval cues may actually hurt recall performance, one study by Kramer et al. showed that intrusions are most commonly associated with cue-recall tasks.[53] This study suggests that cues may lead to intrusions because patients may have a difficult time distinguishing between cues and distractions,[53] which may help explain why cues increase memory errors in patients with Alzheimer’s. Since verbal intrusions are a common aspect of Alzheimer’s,[54] some researchers believe that this characteristic may be helpful in the diagnosis of the disease.[55]

Depression[edit]

Memory errors can occur in patients with depression or with depressive symptoms. Patients with depressive symptoms have a tendency to experience what is known as the negative triad, which is the perspective use of negative schemas and self-concepts to relate to the external world. Due to this negative triad, depressive patients have a tendency to have a much greater focus on, and recall for, negative details and events over positive ones. This may lead to memory errors related to the positive details of memories, impairing the accuracy or even complete recall of such memories. Depressed patients also experience deficits in psychomotor speed and in free recall of material both immediate and delayed. This suggests that material has been encoded but that patients are particularly impaired with regard to search and retrieval processes.[56] Diverse aspects of memory such as short-term memory, long-term memory, semantic memory and implicit memory, have been studied and linked to depression. Short-term memory, a temporary store for newly acquired information, seems to show no major impairments in the case of depressive patients who do seem to complain about poor concentration, which by itself can cause simple memory errors.[56]

Long-term memory, large capacity able to retain information over long periods of time, does however show impairment in the case of depressed individuals. They tend to have difficulties in recall and recognition for both verbal and visuo-spatial material with intervals of a few minutes or even hours creating complex memory errors in relation to speech and surrounding details.[56] Individuals suffering with depression also showed a specific deficit in retrieving information meaningfully organized in their semantic memory, conceptual knowledge about the real world.[56] Therefore, depressive patients can show memory errors for the most meaningful events in their lives, unable to recall these specific moments vividly like someone not suffering from depression might. In the case of implicit memory, when previous information influences ongoing responses, there has been little to no proof of a deficit in relations to depressed individuals.[56]

Schizophrenia[edit]

Memory errors, specifically intrusion errors and imagination inflation effects, have been found to occur in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia. Intrusion errors can commonly be found in the recall portion of a memory test when a participant includes items that were not on the original list that was presented.[57] These types of errors are linked to problems with self-monitoring, increased positive and disorganized symptoms (confusion within the brain), and poor executive functioning.[57] Intrusion errors are found to be more likely in patients with positive schizophrenic symptoms, which involve an excess of normal bodily functions (e.g. delusions), versus negative schizophrenic symptoms, which involve a decrease in normal bodily functions (e.g. refusal to speak).[58] Possible reasons for this are reduced function in the central executive of the working memory, as well as defects in self-reflectivity, organization and reasoning. Self-reflectivity is the ability to recognize and reason about one’s own thought process, recognize that one has thoughts, and that those thoughts are one’s own and differentiate between cognitive operations.[57] Self-reflectivity has been shown to be one of the biggest deficits faced by schizophrenics and data suggests that verbal memory intrusions are linked to deficits in the ability to identify, organize, and reason about one’s own thoughts in patients with schizophrenia.[57]

Imagination inflation effects were also common memory errors in patients with schizophrenia. This effect refers to events that individuals have imagined so vividly in their minds that this adds belief to the fact that the event truly occurred, although it did not. Possible reasons for this are increased source confusion and/or decreased source recollection of an event, which shows poor use of source-monitoring processes.[59] Source-monitoring processes allow one to distinguish between a memory that we may believe has happened because it seems familiar and one that has truly occurred. In the case of schizophrenics, whose abilities to reason through their thoughts is impaired, something that they have imagined and thus, seems familiar can easily be mistaken for an actual event, especially in the case of quick heuristic processes and snap judgments.[59] Continuously imagining an action or event makes this action more and more familiar thus making it harder for a patient with schizophrenia to distinguish its source, questioning whether it is familiar because they have imagined it or if it is familiar because it happened. This leads to many memory errors for these individuals who are led to believe by their imagination of the event that it is real, has occurred and thus is stored in their memory for that reason.[59]

Consequences of memory errors[edit]

Memory errors often lead to the belief of new memories, which are problematic. In the case of eyewitness testimony new false memories can often lead to wrong information and lack of conviction or wrongful conviction of individuals. Also in the case of child abuse, memory errors can lead to the creation of false traumatic childhood memories, which can lead to false accusations and loss of trust.

Eyewitness testimony[edit]

Memory errors can occur in eyewitness testimonies due to a number of features commonly present in a trial, all of which may influence the authenticity of the memory, and may be detrimental to the outcome of the case at hand.
Such features include:

Leading questions
refers to how wording of questions can influence how an event is remembered. This can result from a misinformation effect or an imagination inflation effect.[60] The misinformation effect occurs when information is presented after the events in question have occurred which leads to memory errors in later retrieval.[61] Studies have suggested that witnesses may misattribute accuracy to misleading information because the sources of misleading information and witnessed information become confused.[61] Misleading information can be acquired through reading of the newspaper, watching the news, being interviewed or when sitting in the courthouse during the trial. When witnesses are asked to recall specific details of an event they can begin to doubt their memory, which can cause memory errors. Misinformation can manifest itself as a memory leading the individual to believe it as true and witnesses may also begin to doubt their own memories of the event deciding instead that they must be wrong.[61] Memory errors also occur through the imagination inflation effect. As stated earlier, the imagination inflation effect takes place when an individual imagines an event to the point where it is believed as a memory of an actual event.[60] During trial, witnesses hear many different possible occurrences of events and are led to imagine these situations. Through imagining and rehearsal of the occurrences, witnesses may begin to see vividness and validity in the stories simply from rehearsal, not factual memories.[60] This can create problems for witnesses when trying to distinguish between imagined events and the actual occurrence of the events. Small but largely significant details become easily mixed and these occurrences of memory errors can make or break a trial.
Weapons Focus Effect
refers to the fact that witnesses are highly likely to pay close attention to the weapon being used during an event, which creates a reduction in the ability to remember other details regarding the crime.[62] This can in turn create memory errors leaving the witness less able to remember details such as what the assailant was wearing or what distinctive features could be found on their body or face. One explanation for why witnesses tend to gravitate toward the weapon being used is said to be that the arousal of the witness is increased.[63] When arousal becomes increased the number of perceptional cues being utilized by the brain decreases.[63] This allows the individual to focus on the weapon cue and ignore other cues such as distinct scars or a bright red shirt. The weapons focus effect can also refer to how the report of the use of weapons in the case can influence the memory of the event, leading to a false memory of having heard a weapon being fired even if the witness did not.[63] For example, if a newspaper reports that the victim was beaten with a hammer, upon reading this, the witness will begin to believe that a hammer was in fact used, even if they at no point saw a hammer. This can cause many memory errors and conflict of stories for witnesses. As a society we believe that newspapers or televised news reports have fact behind them. If they report the hammer being used, a witness might begin to second-guess their memory wondering if they missed the hammer or failed to remember that detail.[62] Also, their story may become mixed with the media’s representation of the story and the knife that they did see will be forgotten and instead be replaced with the hammer that was reported.[63]
Familiarity Effect
refers to the tendency of individuals to develop a preference for things simply because they are familiar.[64] This can leave an individual to identify familiar people as guilty, even if they are not. When we are continuously exposed to the same object or person we begin to develop a positive attraction towards said object or person. Simply their familiarity creates a positive sense when re-exposed to the individual or object. In reality one can know very little about a person but by seeing them over and over again can gain unconscious positive recall for their face. This can create memory errors when individuals are asked to identify a criminal and someone familiar to them is placed in the line up. When a familiar face is in among the individuals that the witness is being asked to study, the witness will find himself or herself gravitating towards the familiar face whether or not this is who they witnessed committing the crime. This leaves them more likely to ignore the cues that are leading them towards other individuals and concentrate on the familiar face, resulting in a false accusation. The sense of familiarity can play a large role in the identification of criminals but when the familiarity of a criminal is mixed in with the familiarity of other individuals, choosing the right person can become quite difficult.

Child abuse[edit]

Memory errors regarding the recovery of repressed childhood abuse can occur due to post-event suggestions from a trusted source, such as a family member, or more commonly, a mental health professional. Due to possible relationships between childhood abuse and mental illness later in life, some mental health professionals believe in the Freudian theory of repressed memories as a defense mechanism for the anxiety that recall of the abuse would cause. Freud said that repression operates unconsciously in individuals who are not able to recall a threatening situation or may even forget that the abusive individual was ever part of their lives. Therefore, mental health professionals will sometimes seek to uncover possible instances of childhood abuse in patients, which may lead to suggestibility and cause a false memory of childhood abuse to arise, in an attempt to seek a cause to a mental illness.[65] No matter the confidence in the memory, this does not necessarily equate to the memory being true. This is an example of the misinformation effect and false memory effect. The fact that memories are not retrieved as whole entities but rather are reconstructed from information remaining in memory and other related knowledge make them easily susceptible to memory errors.[66] This explains why working with mental health professionals and leading questions can sometimes manifest false memories by creating knowledge of possible events and asking individuals to focus on if these events actually took place.[67] Individuals begin to overthink these situations visualizing them in their mind and overanalyzing them. This in turn leads to the belief of situations and vivid memories. Patients are left with memories they believe are real and new events from their childhood which can lead to stress and trauma in their adult life and loss of relationships with those who are believed to be the abuser.

See also[edit]

  • Amnesia
  • False memory syndrome
  • Memory implantation
  • Memory loss
  • Memory and aging
  • Memory bias
  • Memory conformity
  • Kleptomnesia

References[edit]

  1. ^ a b Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814
  2. ^ a b c d e f Schacter, Daniel L. (2011). Psychology Second Edition. New York: Worth Publishers. pp. 246. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
  3. ^ a b c d Schacter, Daniel (2011). Psychology 2nd Ed. New York: Worth Publishers. p. 243.
  4. ^ Schacter, Daniel L.,[«Psychology 2nd Ed.»],»Worth Publishers»,2009,2011.
  5. ^ a b Schacter, Daniel L., Daniel T. Gilbert, and Daniel M. Wegner. «Chapter 6: Memory.» Psychology. ; Second Edition. N.p.: Worth, Incorporated, 2011. 244-45.
  6. ^ Schacter, Daniel L., Daniel T. Gilbert, and Daniel M. Wegner. «Chapter 6: Memory.» Psychology. ; Second Edition. N.p.: Worth, Incorporated, 2011. 245.
  7. ^ a b c d e f Loftus, E. (1997). Creating false memories. Scientific American, 277, 70–75
  8. ^ a b c Johnson, M. & Raye, C. (1998). False memories and confabulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(4), 137–145
  9. ^ a b c d e f g Schacter, Daniel L. (2011). Psychology Second Edition. New York: Worth Publishers. pp. 253–254. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
  10. ^ Loftus, E. F. & Hoffman, H. G. (1989). Misinformation and memory, the creation of new memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 118(1), 100–104
  11. ^ Loftus, E. F. & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585–589
  12. ^ a b Deffenbacher, K.A., Bornstein, B.H., & Penrod, S.D. (2006). Mugshot exposure effects: Retroactive interference, mugshot commitment, source confusion, and unconscious transference. Law and Human Behaviour, 30(3), 287–307
  13. ^ Daniel L. Schacter, «Searching for memory: the brain, the mind, and the past», (New York, 1996)
  14. ^ Mazzoni, G. & Memon, A. (2003). Imagination can create false autobiographical memories. Psychological Science, 14(2), 186–188
  15. ^ Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22
  16. ^ a b c d Kleider, H.M., Pezdek, K., Goldinger, S.D., & Kirk, A. (2008). Schema-driven source misattribution errors: Remembering the expected from a witnessed event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1–20
  17. ^ Jacobs, D. (1990). Intrusion errors in the figural memory of patients with Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, 49–57.
  18. ^ a b c d Stip, E., Corbière, M., Boulay, L. J., Lesage, A., Lecomte, T., Leclerc, C., Richard, N., Cyr, M., & Guillem, F. (2007). Intrusion errors in explicit memory: Their differential relationship with clinical and social outcome in chronic schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 12(2), 112–127
  19. ^ a b c Kahana et al. (2006). Temporal Associations and Prior list intrusions in Free Recall.
  20. ^ Smith, Troy, A., Kimball, Daniel, R. Kahana, Michael, J. (2007). The fSAM Model of False Recall.
  21. ^ Wickelgren, Wayne, A., (1965. Similarity and Intrusions in Short Term Memory for Consonant-Vowel Digrams.
  22. ^ Wickelgren, Wayne, A., (1965). Acoustic Similarity and Intrusion Errors in Short Term Memory.
  23. ^ a b c d e Hyman Jr., I.E., & Loftus, E.F. (1998). Errors in autobiographical memory. Clinical Psychology Review, 18(8), 933–947
  24. ^ a b Greenberg, D.L. (2004). President Bush’s false «flashbulb» memory of 9/11/01. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 363–370
  25. ^ Laney, C., & Loftus, E.F. (2010). Truth in emotional memories. In B.H. Bornstein (Ed.), Emotion and the Law (pp. 157–183). Leicester: Springer Science + Business Media
  26. ^ a b c Wade, K.A., & Garry, M. (2005). Strategies for verifying false autobiographical memories. American Journal of Psychology, 118(4), 587–602
  27. ^ Mojet J, Ko¨ster EP. Sensory memory and food texture. Food Qual Prefer 2005; 16: 251–266. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2004.04.017.
  28. ^ a b Dell, G.S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93(3), 283–321
  29. ^ Anderson, J.R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 261–295
  30. ^ a b Kapatsinski, V. (2004). Frequency, age-of-acquisition, lexicon size, neighborhood density, and speed of processing: towards a domain-general, single mechanism account. High Desert Linguistic Society Conference, 6, 131–150.
  31. ^ a b Forster, K.I. (1991). The density constraint on form-priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(1), 1–25.
  32. ^ a b c Jaimes, A., Omura, K., Nagamine, T., & Hirata, K. (2004). Memory cues for meeting video retrieval. In
    Proceedings of the 1st ACM Workshop on Continuous Archival and Retrieval of Personal Experiences
    (CARPE) at the ACM International Multimedia Conference
  33. ^ Tulving, E., & Thomson, D.M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352–373
  34. ^ Smith, S.M. (1984). A comparison of two techniques for reducing context-dependent forgetting. Memory and Cognition, 12(5), 477–482.
  35. ^ Eich, J.E. (1980). The cue-dependent nature of state-dependent retrieval. Memory and Cognition, 8(2), 157–173.
  36. ^ Thomson, D.M., & Tulving, E. (1970). Associative encoding and retrieval: Weak and strong cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86(2), 255–262.
  37. ^ a b Moscovitc, M., & Craik, F.I.M. (1976). Depth of processing, retrieval cues, and uniqueness of encoding as factors in recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 447–458.
  38. ^ Baddeley, A.D., & Dale, H.C.A. (1966). The effect of semantic similarity on retro-active interference in long- and short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 417–420.
  39. ^ Badre, D. & Wagner, A.D. (2005). Frontal lobe mechanisms that resolve proactive interference. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 2003–2012.
  40. ^ Dewar, M.T., Cowan, N., & Della Sala, S. (2007). Forgetting due to retroactive interference: A fusion of Muller and Pilzecker’s (1900) early insights into everyday forgetting and recent research on anterograde amnesia. Cortex, 43(5), 616–634.
  41. ^ Conway, M. A., & Tacchi, P. C. (1996). Motivated confabulation. Neurocase, 2, 325–338
  42. ^ Treves, A. & Rolls, E. T. (1994). Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory. Neuroscience, 4(3), 374–391
  43. ^ Kensinger, E.A., & Corkin, S. (2004). The effects of emotional content and aging on false memories. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioural Neuroscience, 4(1), 1–9.
  44. ^ a b c d e f g h Schacter, D.L., Koutstaal, W., & Norman, K.A. (1997). False memories and aging. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(6).
  45. ^ Kensinger, E.A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 1(2), 99–113
  46. ^ Dolcos, F., & Denkova, E. (2008). Neural correlates of encoding emotional memories: A review of functional neuroimaging evidence. Cell Science, 5(2)
  47. ^ Schmidt, S.R. (2004). Autobiographical memories for the September 11 attacks: Reconstructive errors and emotional impairment of memory. Memory and Cognition, 32(3), 443–454
  48. ^ Ruci, L., Tomes, J.L., & Zelenski, J.M. (2009). Mood-congruent false memories in the DRM paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 23(6), 1153–1165
  49. ^ Baddeley, A.D., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., Logie, R., & Spinnler, H. (1991). The decline of working memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 114, 2521–2542.
  50. ^ Mimura, M., & Komatsu, S.I. (2010). Factors of error and effort in memory intervention for patients with Alzheimer’s disease and amnesic syndrome. Psychogeriatrics, 10, 179–186.
  51. ^ a b Mitchell, J.P., Sullivan, A.L., Schacter, D.L., & Budson, A.E. (2006). Misattribution errors in Alzheimer’s disease: The illusory truth effect. Neuropsychology, 20(2), 185–192.
  52. ^ Hildebrandt, H., Haldenwanger, A., & Eling, P. (2009). False recognition helps to distinguish patients with Alzheimer’s disease and amnesic mci from patients with other kinds of dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 28(2).
  53. ^ a b Kramer, J.H., Delis, D.C., Blusewicz, M.J., & Brandt, J. (1988). Verbal memory errors in Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s dementias. Developmental Neuropsychology, 4(1), 1–15.
  54. ^ Kern, B.S., Gorp, W.G.V., Cummings, J.L., Brown, W.S., & Osato, S.S. (1992). Confabulation in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 19(2), 172–182.
  55. ^ Fuld, P.A., Katzman, R., Davies, P., & Terry, R.D. (2004). Intrusions as a sign of Alzheimer dementia chemical and pathological verification. Annals of Neurology, 11(2), 155–159.
  56. ^ a b c d e Ilsley, J.E., Moffoot, A.P.R., & O’Carroll, R.E. (1995). An analysis of memory dysfunction in major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 35, 1–9.
  57. ^ a b c d Fridberg, D.J., Brenner, A., & Lysaker, P.H. (2010). Verbal memory intrusions in schizophrenia: Associations with self-reflectivity, symptomatology, and neurocognition. Psychiatry Research, 179, 6–11.
  58. ^ Brébion, G., Amador, X., Smith, M.J., Malaspina, D., Sharif, Z. & Gorman, J.M. (1999). Opposite links of positive and negative symptomatology with memory errors in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 88, 15–24.
  59. ^ a b c Mammarella, N., Altamura M., Padalino F.A., Petito A., Fairfield B. & Bellomo A. (2010). False memories in schizophrenia? An imagination inflation study. Psychiatry Research, 179, 267–273.
  60. ^ a b c Whitehouse, W.G., Orne, E.C., Dinges, D.F. (2010). Extreme cognitive interviewing: A blueprint for false memories through imagination inflation. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 58(3), 269–287
  61. ^ a b c English, S.M., Nielson, K.A. (2010). Reduction of the misinformation effect by arousal induced after learning. Cognition, 117(2), 237–242.
  62. ^ a b Loftus, E.F., Loftus, G.R., Messo, J. (1987). Some fact about “weapon focus”. Law and Human Behavior, 11(1), 55–62.
  63. ^ a b c d Mitchell, K.J., Livosky, M., Mather, M. (1998). The weapon focus effect revisited: The role of novelty. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 287–303
  64. ^ Moreland, R.L., Zajonc, R.B. (1982). Exposure effects in person perception: Familiarity, similarity, and attraction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 18(5), 395–415
  65. ^ Lindsay, D.S., & Read, J.D. (1994). Psychotherapy and memories of childhood sexual abuse: A cognitive perspective. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 281–338.
  66. ^ Hyman Jr., I.E., & Pentland, J. (1996). The role of mental imagery in the creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 101–117.
  67. ^ Lindsay, D.S., & Read, J.D. (1995). Memory work and recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse: Scientific evidence and public, professional, and personal issues. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 1(4), 846–908

Memory gaps and errors refer to the incorrect recall, or complete loss, of information in the memory system for a specific detail and/or event. Memory errors may include remembering events that never occurred, or remembering them differently from the way they actually happened.[1] These errors or gaps can occur due to a number of different reasons, including the emotional involvement in the situation, expectations and environmental changes. As the retention interval between encoding and retrieval of the memory lengthens, there is an increase in both the amount that is forgotten, and the likelihood of a memory error occurring.

Overview[edit]

There are several different types of memory errors, in which people may inaccurately recall details of events that did not occur, or they may simply misattribute the source of a memory. In other instances, imagination of a certain event can create confidence that such an event actually occurred. Causes of such memory errors may be due to certain cognitive factors, such as spreading activation, or to physiological factors, including brain damage, age or emotional factors. Furthermore, memory errors have been reported in individuals with schizophrenia and depression. The consequences of memory errors can have significant implications. Two main areas of concern regarding memory errors are in eyewitness testimony and cases of child abuse.

Types of memory errors[edit]

Blocking[edit]

The feeling that a person gets when they know the information, but can not remember a specific detail, like an individual’s name or the name of a place is described as the «tip-of-the-tongue» experience. The tip-of-the-tongue experience is a classic example of blocking, which is a failure to retrieve information that is available in memory even though you are trying to produce it.[2] The information you are trying to remember has been encoded and stored, and a cue is available that would usually trigger its recollection.[2] The information has not faded from memory and a person is not forgetting to retrieve the information.[2] What a person is experiencing is a complete retrieval failure, which makes blocking especially frustrating.[2] Blocking occurs especially often for the names of people and places, because their links to related concepts and knowledge are weaker than for common names.[2] The experience of blocking occurs more often as we get older; this «tip of the tongue» experience is a common complaint amongst 60- and 70-year-olds.[2]

Transience[edit]

Transience refers to forgetting what occurs with the passage of time.[3] Transience occurs during the storage phase of memory, after an experience has been encoded and before it is retrieved.[3] As time passes, the quality of our memory also changes, deteriorating from specific to more general.[3] German philosopher named Hermann Ebbinghaus decided to measure his own memory for lists of nonsense syllables at various times after studying them. He decided to draw out a curve of his forgetting pattern over time. He realized that there is a rapid drop-off in retention during the first tests and there is a slower rate of forgetting later on.[3] Therefore, transience denotes the gradual change of a specific knowledge or idea into more general memories.[4]

Absentmindedness[edit]

Absentmindedness is a gap in attention which causes memory failure. In this situation the information does not disappear from memory, it can later be recalled. But the lack of attention at a specific moment prevents the information from being recalled at that specific moment. A common cause of absentmindedness is a lack of attention.[clarification needed] Attention is vital to encoding information in long-term memory. Without proper attention, material is much less likely to be stored properly and recalled later.[5] When attention is divided, less activity in the lower left frontal lobe diminishes the ability for elaborative memory encoding to take place, and results in absentminded forgetting. More recent research has shown that divided attention also leads to less hippocampal involvement in encoding.[5] A common example of absentmindedness is not remembering to carry out actions that had been planned to be done in the future, for example, picking up grocery items, and remembering times of meetings.[6]

False memories[edit]

False memories, sometimes referred to as confabulation, refer to the recollection of inaccurate details of an event, or recollection of a whole event that never occurred. Studies investigating this memory error have been able to successfully implant memories among participants that never existed, such as being lost in a mall as a child (termed the lost in the mall technique) or spilling a bowl of punch at a wedding reception.[7] In this case, false memories were implanted among participants by their family members who claimed that the event had happened. This evidence demonstrates the possibility of implanting false memories on individuals by leading them to remember such events that never occurred. This memory error can be particularly worrisome in judicial settings, in which witnesses may have false recollections of a crime after it has happened, especially when told by others that particular things may have happened which did not.[8] Another area of concern regarding false memories is in cases of child abuse.

Problem of bias[edit]

The problem of bias, which is the distorting influences of present knowledge, beliefs, and feelings on recollection of previous experiences.[9] Sometimes what people remember from their past says less about what actually happened than about what they personally believe, feel, and the knowledge they have acquired at the present time.[9] An individual’s current moods can bias their memory recall, researchers have found.[9] There are three types of memory biases, consistency bias, change bias and egocentric bias.[9] Consistency bias is the bias to reconstruct the past to fit the present.[9] Change bias is the tendency to exaggerate differences between what we feel or believe in the present and what we previously felt or believed in the past.[9] Egocentric bias is a form of change bias, the tendency to exaggerate the change between the past and the present in order to make ourselves look good in any given situation.[9]

Misinformation effect[edit]

The misinformation effect refers to the change in memory due to the presentation of information that is relevant to the target memory, such as leading questions or suggestions.[10] Memories are likely to be altered when questions are worded differently or when inaccurate information is presented. For example, in one experiment participants watched a video of an automobile accident and were then asked questions regarding the accident. When asked how fast the automobiles were driving when they smashed into each other, the speed estimate was higher than when asked how fast the automobiles were driving when they hit, bumped or collided into each other. Similarly, participants were more likely to report there being shattered glass present when the word smashed was used instead of other verbs.[11] Evidently, memory recollection can be altered with misleading information after the event.

Source confusion[edit]

Source confusion or unconscious transference,[12] involves the misattribution of the source of a memory. For instance, an individual may recall seeing an event in person when in reality they only witnessed the event on television.[12] Ultimately, the individual has an inability to remember the source of information in which the content and the source become dissociated. This may be more likely for more distant memories, such as childhood memories.[7] In more severe cases of source confusion, you can take fictional stories you heard from when you were younger and assimilated these stories being your childhood. For example, say your father told you stories about his life when he was a child every night before you went to sleep when you were a child. When you grow up, you might mistakenly remember these stories your dad told you as your own and integrate them into your childhood memories.[13]

Imagination inflation[edit]

Imagination inflation refers to when a person remembers details of a memory that are exaggerated versions of the actual event or remember an entire memory that never occurred due to the act of imagination.[14] That is, when one imagines an event occurring, their confidence that this event actually did occur increases. One reason for this may be due to the act of imagination increasing the familiarity of the event. When the event seems more familiar, it may become more likely for people to report it actually occurring. For instance, in an experiment participants were asked to imagine playing inside and then running outside toward a window, falling and breaking it, while other participants did not imagine anything. Participants who had imagined this scenario reported an increased level of confidence that the event had actually happened in comparison to those who did not imagine the event.[7] This error can be caused simply by imagining an event.

Deese–Roediger–McDermott (DRM) paradigm[edit]

Deese-Roediger-McDermott paradigm refers to the incorrect recall of features of an event that were not actually present, due to the features being related to a common theme.[15] This paradigm has been demonstrated with the use of word lists and subsequent recognition tests. For example, experiments have shown that if a research participant is presented with the words: bed rest awake tired dream wake snooze snore nap yawn drowsy, there is a high likelihood that the participant will falsely recall that the word sleep was in the list of words. These results show a significant illusion in memory, in which people remember items that were never presented simply due to their relation with other items in a common theme.[1]

Schematic errors[edit]

Schematic errors refer to the use of a schema to help reconstruct parts of an experience that cannot be remembered. This may include parts of the schema that did not actually take place, or aspects of a schema that are stereotypical of an event.[16] Schemas can be described as mental guidelines (scripts) for events that are encountered in daily life.[16] For example, when going to the gas station, there is a general pattern of how things will occur (i.e. turn car off, get out of car, open gas tank, punch the gas button, put nozzle into the tank, fill up the tank, put the nozzle back, close the tank, pay, turn car on, leave). Schemas make the world more predictable, allowing expectations to be formed of how things will enfold and to take note of things that happen out of context.[16] However, schemas also allow for memory errors, in that if certain aspects of a scene or event are missing from memory, people may incorrectly remember having actually seen or experienced them because they are usually a regular aspect of the schema. For example, an individual may not remember paying the waiter, but may believe they have done so, as this is a regular step in the script of going to a restaurant. Similarly, a person may recall seeing a fridge in a picture of a kitchen, even if one was not actually depicted due to existing schemas which suggest that kitchens almost always contain a fridge.[16]

Intrusion errors[edit]

Intrusion errors refer to when information that is related to the theme of a certain memory, but was not actually a part of the original episode, become associated with the event.[17] This makes it difficult to distinguish which elements are in fact part of the original memory. One idea regarding how intrusion errors work is due to a lack of recall inhibition, which allows irrelevant information to be brought to awareness while attempting to remember.[18] Another possible explanation is that intrusion errors result from a lack of new context integration into a viable memory trace, or into an already existing memory trace that is related to the appropriate memory.[18] More explanations involve the temporal aspect of recall, meaning that as the time difference between the study periods of different lists approaches zero, the amount of intrusions between the lists tends to increase,[19] the semantic aspect, meaning that the list of target words may have induced a false recall of non-target words that happen to have a similar or same meaning as the targets,[20] and the similarity aspect, for example subjects who were given list of letters to recall were likely to replace target vowels with non-target vowels.[21]

Intrusion errors can be divided into two categories. The first are known as extra-list errors, which occur when incorrect and non-related items are recalled, and were not part of the word study list.[18] These types of intrusion errors often follow the DRM Paradigm effects, in which the incorrectly recalled items are often thematically related to the study list one is attempting to recall from. Another pattern for extra-list intrusions would be an acoustic similarity pattern, this pattern states that targets that have a similar sound to non-targets may be replaced with those non-targets in recall.[22] One major type of extra-list intrusions is called the «Prior-List Intrusion» (PLI), a PLI occurs when targets from previously studied lists are recalled instead of the targets in the current list. PLIs often follow under the temporal aspect of intrusions in that since they were recalled recently they have a high chance of being recalled now.[19] The second type of intrusion error is known as intra-list errors, which is similar to extra-list errors, except it refers to irrelevant recall for items that were on the word study list.[18] Although these two categories of intrusion errors are based on word list studies in laboratories, the concepts can be extrapolated to real-life situations. Also, the same three factors that play a critical role in correct recall (recency, temporal association and semantic relatedness) play a role in intrusions as well.[19]

Time-slice errors[edit]

Time-slice errors occur when a correct event is in fact recalled; however the event that was asked to be recalled is not the one that is recalled. In other words, the timing of events is incorrectly remembered.[23] As discovered in a study by Brewer (1988), often the event or event details that are recalled occurred within a short time proximity to the memory required to be recalled.[23] There are three possible theories as to why time-slice errors occur. First, they may be a form of interference, in which the memory information from one time impairs the recall of information from a different time.[24] (see interference below). A second theory is that intrusion errors may be responsible, in that memories revolving around a similar time period thus share a common theme, and memories of various points of time within that larger time period become mixed with each other and intrude on each other’s recall. Last, the recall of memories often have holes due to forgotten details. Thus, individuals may be using a script (see schema errors) to help fill in these holes with general knowledge of what they know happened around this time. Since scripts are a time-based knowledge structure, which puts details of a memory in sequence to make it easier to understand, time-slice errors can occur if a detail is mistakenly placed in the wrong sequence.[24]

Personal life effects[edit]

Personal life effects refer to the recall and belief in events as declared by family members or friends for having actually happened.[25] Personal life effects are largely based on suggestive influences from external sources, such as family members or a therapist.[7] Other influential sources may include media or literature stories which involve details that are believed to have been experienced or witnessed, such as a natural disaster close to where one resides, or a situation that is common and could have occurred, such as getting lost as a child. Personal life effects are most powerful when claimed to be true by a family member, and even more powerful when a secondary source confirms the event having happened.[7]

Personal life effects are believed to be a form of source confusion, in which the individual cannot recall where the memory is coming from.[26] Therefore, without being able to confirm the source of the memory, the individual may accept the false memory as true. Three factors may be responsible for the implantation of false autobiographical memories. The first factor is time. As time passes, memories fade. Therefore, source confusion may result due to time delay.[7] The second factor is the imagination inflation effect. As the amount of imagination increases, so does one’s familiarity for the contents of the imagination. Thus, source confusion may also occur due to the individual confusing the source of the memory for being true, when in fact it was imaginary.[26] Lastly, social pressure to recall the memory may affect the individual’s belief in the false memory. For example, with increase in pressure, the individual may lower their criteria for validating a memory, and come to accept a false memory for being true.[26] Personal life effects can be extremely crucial to recognize in cases of recovered memories, especially those of abuse, in which the individual may have been led to believe they had been abused as a child by a therapist during psychological therapy, when in fact they had not been. Personal life effects can also be important in witness testimonies, in which suggestions from authorities may incorrectly implant memories regarding witnesses a particular detail about a crime (see the Childhood Abuse and Eye Witness Testimony sections below).

Memory error relating to food[edit]

In a study done by J. Mojet and E.P. Köster, participants were asked to have a drink, yoghurt, and some biscuits for breakfast. A few hours later, they were asked to identify and the items they had during breakfast out of five variations of the drinks, yoghurts, and biscuits. The results showed that participants remember the texture of the foods much better than for fatty content, although they could discern the difference of both among the different items. Participants were also most certain about foods that they did not have during breakfast, but were the least certain about foods that they said were in their breakfast and about foods that were in their breakfast but were not recognized. This suggests that incidental and implicit memory for foods are more focused on discerning new and potentially unsafe food rather than remembering the details of familiar foods.[27]

Causes[edit]

Cognitive factors[edit]

Spreading activation[edit]

One theory on how memory works is through a concept called spreading activation. Spreading activation refers to the firing of connected nodes in associated memory links.[28] The theory states that memory is organized in a theoretical web of associated ideas and related features. Each feature, or node, is connected to related features or ideas, which in turn are themselves connected to other related nodes.[29]

Spreading activation can also demonstrate how memory errors may occur. As the network of memory associations increases in the number of connections–the connection density–the likelihood of memory gaps and errors occurring also increases. Put simply, the amount of activation a secondary connection receives depends on how many connections the initial node has associated with it. This is because the initial node must divide the amount of activation it spreads to related nodes by the number of connecting nodes it is associated with. If node 1 has three connecting nodes, and node 2 has 15 connecting nodes, the three connecting nodes from node 1 will receive greater activation levels (the activation level is divided less) than for the 15 connecting nodes from node 2, and the components of these nodes will be more easily brought to mind. What this means is that, with more connections a node has, the more difficulty there is bringing to mind one of its connecting features.[30] This can lead to memory errors, in that if the connection density is so great that there is not enough activation reaching the secondary nodes, then the person may not recall a target memory that is actually present, and a memory error occurs.

Activation levels to secondary nodes is also determined by the strength of the association to the primary node.[28] Some connections have greater association with the primary node (e.g. fire truck and fire or red, versus fire truck and hose or Dalmatian), and thus will receive a greater portion of the divided activation than less associated connections. Thus, associations that receive less activation will be out-competed by associations with stronger connections, and may fail to be brought into awareness, again causing a memory error.

Connection density[edit]

The connection densities, or neighbourhood densities[31] of memory arrangements help distinguish which elements are a part of, or related to, the target memory. As the density of neural networks increases, the number of retrieval cues (associated nodes) also increases, which may allow for enhanced memory of the event.[30] However, too many connections can inhibit memory in two ways. First, as described under the sub-section Spreading Activation, the total activation being spread from node 1 to connecting nodes is divided by the number of connections. With a greater number of connections, each connecting node receives less activation, which may result in too little activation for the memory cue to be brought to awareness. Connection strength, in which more strongly connected associations receive more activation than less-related associations, may also prevent specific connections from being brought to awareness due to being out-competed by the stronger associations.[31] Second, with more connections branching from various other nodes, there is a greater probability of linking associated connections of different memories together (transplant errors) so that memory errors occur and incorrect features are recalled.

Retrieval cues[edit]

A retrieval cue is a type of hint that can be used to evoke a memory that has been stored but cannot be recalled. Retrieval cues work by selecting traces or associations in memory that contain specific content. With regards to the theory of spreading activation, retrieval cues use associated nodes to help activate a specific or target node.[32] When no cues are available, recall is greatly reduced, leading to forgetting and possible memory errors. This is called retrieval failure, or cue-dependent forgetting.

Retrieval cues can be divided into two subsets, although they are by no means used independent of each other. The first are called feature cues, in which information of the content of the original memory, or related content, is used to aid recall.[32] The second category is context cues, in which information based on the specific context (environment) in which the memory or learning occurred is used to aid recall.[32]

Although retrieval cues are usually helpful in helping to remember something, they can produce memory errors in three ways. First, incorrect cues can be used, leading to recall of an incorrect memory. Second, although the correct retrieval cues may be used, they may still produce an incorrect memory. This is likely to occur with high connection densities, in which the incorrect (but associated) node was activated and thus recalled, instead of the target memory. Third, the retrieval cue chosen may be correct and associated with the target memory, but it may not have a strong connection to the target memory, and thus may not produce enough activation to produce the target memory.

Encoding specificity[edit]

Encoding specificity is when retrieval is successful to the extent that the retrieval cues used to help recall, match the cues the individual used during learning or encoding.[33] Memory errors due to encoding specificity means that the memory is likely not forgotten, however, the specific cues used during encoding the primary event are now unavailable to help remember the event. The cues used during encoding are dependent on the environment of the individual at the time the memory occurred. In context-dependent memory, recall is based on the difference between the encoding and recall environments.[34] Recall for items learned in a particular context is better when recall occurs in the same place as when the initial memory occurred. This is why it is sometimes useful to “return to the scene of the crime” to help witnesses remember details of a crime, or for explaining why going to a specific location such as a residence or community setting, may lead to becoming flooded with memories of things that happened in that context. Recall can also depend on state-dependency, or the conditions of one’s internal environment, at both the time of the event and of recall.[35] For example, if intoxicated at the time the memory actually occurred, recall for details of the event is greater when recalling while intoxicated. Associated with state-dependency, recall can also depend on mood-dependency, in which recall is greater when the mood for when the memory occurred matches the mood during recall.[23] These specific dependencies are based on the fact that the cues used during the initial event can be specific to the context or state one was in. In other words, various features of the environment (both internal and external) can be used to help encode the memory, and thus become retrieval cues. However, if the context or state changes at the time of recall, these cues may no longer be available, and thus recall is hindered.

Transfer-appropriate processing[edit]

Memory errors can also depend on the method of encoding used when initially experiencing or learning information, known as transfer-appropriate processing.[36] Encoding processes can occur at three levels: visual form (the letters that make up a word), phonology (the sound of a word), and semantics (the meaning of the word or sentence). With relation to memory errors, the level of processing at the time of encoding and at the time of recall should be the same.[37] Although semantic processing generally produces greater recall that shallower levels of processing, a study by Morris et al. demonstrated that what might be the key factor to greater recall is transfer-appropriate processing–when the level of processing at the original memory/learning time matches the level of processing used to help recall. In other words, if learning occurred by rhyming the target words to other words, then recall is best if testing is also at the phonological level of processing, such as a rhyming recognition test. Thus, memory errors can occur when the levels of processing between encoding and recall do not match.[37]

Interference[edit]

Interference occurs when specific information inhibits learning and /or recall for a specific memory.[38] There are two forms of interference. First, proactive interference has to do with difficulty in learning new material based on the inability to override information from older memories.[39] In such cases, retrieval cues continue to be associated and aimed at recalling previously learned information, affecting the recall of new material. Retroactive interference is the opposite of proactive interference, in which there is difficulty in the recall of previously learned information based on the interference of newly acquired information. In this case, retrieval cues are associated with the new information and not the older memory.[40] thus affecting recall of older material. Interference of either form can produce memory errors, in which there is interference with the recall of material. In other words, previously used retrieval cues are no longer associated with prior memories, and thus memory confusion or even an inability to recall the memory can occur.

Physiological factors[edit]

Brain damage[edit]

Lobes of the human brain

Frontal lobe

Temporal lobe

Parietal lobe

Occipital
lobe

Damage in the temporal lobe (green) and frontal lobe (blue) are associated with resulting memory errors

Neuroimaging studies have provided evidence for links between brain damage and memory errors. Brain areas implicated include the frontal lobe and medial-temporal regions of the brain. Such damage may result in significant confabulations and source confusion.[8]
The prefrontal cortex is in charge of making heuristic judgments and systematic judgments, which involve analyzing the qualities of memories and the retrieval and evaluation of supporting or disconfirming information.[8] Thus, if the frontal region is damaged, these abilities will be impaired and memories may not be retrieved or evaluated correctly. For example, one patient who suffered frontal lobe damage after an automobile accident reported memories of the support his family provided for him after the accident, which in reality, was false.[41]

The temporal region of the brain contains the hippocampus, which plays a significant role in memory.[42] Thus, damage in this region may impair the function of this brain structure and result in memory problems.

Age[edit]

Studies have shown that the likelihood of memory errors occurring increases as age increases. Possible reasons for this are increased source confusion for the event and findings that older persons have decreased levels of processing when first presented with new information.[43][44] Source confusion refers to the inability to distinguish how one came upon different information. Older individuals can become confused with where they learnt information (e.g. TV, radio, newspaper, word of mouth, etc.), who presented them with the information (e.g. which of two experimenters presented them with facts and which presented them with irrelevant information), and whether the information came from an imagined source, and is thus not factual, or a real world source.[44] This in itself is a form of memory error but can also create larger memory errors. When an older individual is confused whether the information is factual or was imagined they can begin to accept imagined memories as real and thus begin to rely on new false information.[44]

Levels of processing refers to the manner in which someone encodes information into their memory and how deeply it is encoded.[44] There are three different levels of processing ranging from shallow to deep, deep being stored in long-term memory for a longer period and thus better remembered. The three levels are; visual form, being the shallowest form, phonology, being a medium level of processing, and semantics (meaning), which is the deepest form of processing.[44] The visual form of processing relies on the ability to see information and break it down into its components (e.g. see the word «dog», composed D, O, and G). Phonology relies on creating links to information through sound such as cues and tricks for memory (e.g. Dog rhymes with Fog).[44] Finally, semantics refer to the creation of meaning behind information such as adding detail to allow the information to create links throughout our memory with other memories and thus be held in long-term memory for a longer period (e.g. A dog is a four-legged pet that often chases cats and chews on bones).[44] Older individuals often begin to lose the quick ability to add meaning to new information, which leads to shallower processing and easier forgetting of the information gained.[44] Both of these possible factors can decrease the validity of the memory due to retrieval for details of the event being more difficult to perform. This leads to details of memories being confused with others or lost completely, which lead to many errors in recall of a memory.

Emotion[edit]

The emotional impact of an event can have a direct impact on how the memory is first encoded, how it is later recalled, and what details of the event are accurately recalled. Highly emotional events tend to be recalled easily due to their emotional impact,[45] as well as their distinctiveness relative to other memories (highly emotional events do not occur on a regular basis, and thus are easily separated from other events that are more commonly met). Emotional events may affect memory not only because of their distinctiveness, but also because of their arousal on the individual.[46] Studies have found that prime or central features of such highly emotional events tend to be accurately recalled, whereas subtle details of the events are not remembered, or are remembered with vague consistency. Memory errors related to highly emotional events are influenced in ways such as:

  • Whether the event was positive or negative in nature — The nature of the event can affect memory, negative events tend to be remembered with greater accuracy than positive events.[23]
  • Implicit theories of consistency and change — This term was coined by Ross (1989), and is used to describe the phenomena of memory change based on the belief of how the person felt at the time of the event compared to their current feelings about it.[23] In other words, the memory of one’s emotion towards an event can change depending on their current emotional state toward the same event. If a person believes their feelings at both times continue to be the same, then the current emotion to «remember» how they felt about the event at a previous time is used. If feelings are believed to have changed, then recall of the emotional involvement in the past event is adjusted to the current feelings.
  • Intrusion errors — This term refers to the inclusion of details that may have been commonly experienced in the event, but not by the individual. For example, in the September 11 terrorist attacks, many people may state that they remember hearing about the attacks on the television news, as this was a common way of finding out this information, whereas they may have actually heard about it from a neighbour or on the radio.[47]
  • Mood-congruency — Items/events are better recalled when the mood of the individual at the time of the event and the time of recall are the same. Thus, if the mood at the time of recall does not match the mood experienced at the time the event occurred, there is an increased chance that complete recall will be affected/interrupted.[48]

Memory errors in Abnormal Psychology[edit]

Abnormal psychology is the branch of psychology that studies unusual patterns of behavior, emotion and thought, which may or may not be understood as a mental disorder. Memory errors can commonly be found in types of abnormal psychology such as Alzheimer’s disease, depression, and schizophrenia.

Alzheimer’s disease[edit]

Alzheimer’s disease is characterized by progressive memory impairment and decline, usually beginning short-term memory.[49] As it is a progressive disease, Alzheimer’s usually begins with memory errors before proceeding to long-term memory stores. One form of memory error occurs in contrast to the theory of retrieval cues in being a reason for the occurrence of memory errors. As noted above, memory errors may be due to the lack of cues that can trigger a memory trace and bring it to awareness. However, studies have shown that the opposite may be true for patients with Alzheimer’s, such that cues may actually decrease perform on
priming tasks.[50] Patients also demonstrate errors known as misattribution errors, otherwise known as source confusion. However, studies show that these misattribution errors are dependent on whether the task is a familiarity or recollection task.[51]

Although patients tend to exhibit a higher level of false recognitions than control groups,[52] researchers have shown that they may exhibit less false-recognition early in the test due to familiarity being slower to develop. However, the observation of increasing instances of misattribution errors can be seen once familiarity does occur.[51] This may be related to the retrieval cue speculation, in that familiar memories often contain cues we know, and thus may be the reason why familiarity can contribute to memory errors. Lastly, many studies have shown that Alzheimer’s patients commonly suffer from intrusion errors. Relative to the findings that retrieval cues may actually hurt recall performance, one study by Kramer et al. showed that intrusions are most commonly associated with cue-recall tasks.[53] This study suggests that cues may lead to intrusions because patients may have a difficult time distinguishing between cues and distractions,[53] which may help explain why cues increase memory errors in patients with Alzheimer’s. Since verbal intrusions are a common aspect of Alzheimer’s,[54] some researchers believe that this characteristic may be helpful in the diagnosis of the disease.[55]

Depression[edit]

Memory errors can occur in patients with depression or with depressive symptoms. Patients with depressive symptoms have a tendency to experience what is known as the negative triad, which is the perspective use of negative schemas and self-concepts to relate to the external world. Due to this negative triad, depressive patients have a tendency to have a much greater focus on, and recall for, negative details and events over positive ones. This may lead to memory errors related to the positive details of memories, impairing the accuracy or even complete recall of such memories. Depressed patients also experience deficits in psychomotor speed and in free recall of material both immediate and delayed. This suggests that material has been encoded but that patients are particularly impaired with regard to search and retrieval processes.[56] Diverse aspects of memory such as short-term memory, long-term memory, semantic memory and implicit memory, have been studied and linked to depression. Short-term memory, a temporary store for newly acquired information, seems to show no major impairments in the case of depressive patients who do seem to complain about poor concentration, which by itself can cause simple memory errors.[56]

Long-term memory, large capacity able to retain information over long periods of time, does however show impairment in the case of depressed individuals. They tend to have difficulties in recall and recognition for both verbal and visuo-spatial material with intervals of a few minutes or even hours creating complex memory errors in relation to speech and surrounding details.[56] Individuals suffering with depression also showed a specific deficit in retrieving information meaningfully organized in their semantic memory, conceptual knowledge about the real world.[56] Therefore, depressive patients can show memory errors for the most meaningful events in their lives, unable to recall these specific moments vividly like someone not suffering from depression might. In the case of implicit memory, when previous information influences ongoing responses, there has been little to no proof of a deficit in relations to depressed individuals.[56]

Schizophrenia[edit]

Memory errors, specifically intrusion errors and imagination inflation effects, have been found to occur in patients diagnosed with schizophrenia. Intrusion errors can commonly be found in the recall portion of a memory test when a participant includes items that were not on the original list that was presented.[57] These types of errors are linked to problems with self-monitoring, increased positive and disorganized symptoms (confusion within the brain), and poor executive functioning.[57] Intrusion errors are found to be more likely in patients with positive schizophrenic symptoms, which involve an excess of normal bodily functions (e.g. delusions), versus negative schizophrenic symptoms, which involve a decrease in normal bodily functions (e.g. refusal to speak).[58] Possible reasons for this are reduced function in the central executive of the working memory, as well as defects in self-reflectivity, organization and reasoning. Self-reflectivity is the ability to recognize and reason about one’s own thought process, recognize that one has thoughts, and that those thoughts are one’s own and differentiate between cognitive operations.[57] Self-reflectivity has been shown to be one of the biggest deficits faced by schizophrenics and data suggests that verbal memory intrusions are linked to deficits in the ability to identify, organize, and reason about one’s own thoughts in patients with schizophrenia.[57]

Imagination inflation effects were also common memory errors in patients with schizophrenia. This effect refers to events that individuals have imagined so vividly in their minds that this adds belief to the fact that the event truly occurred, although it did not. Possible reasons for this are increased source confusion and/or decreased source recollection of an event, which shows poor use of source-monitoring processes.[59] Source-monitoring processes allow one to distinguish between a memory that we may believe has happened because it seems familiar and one that has truly occurred. In the case of schizophrenics, whose abilities to reason through their thoughts is impaired, something that they have imagined and thus, seems familiar can easily be mistaken for an actual event, especially in the case of quick heuristic processes and snap judgments.[59] Continuously imagining an action or event makes this action more and more familiar thus making it harder for a patient with schizophrenia to distinguish its source, questioning whether it is familiar because they have imagined it or if it is familiar because it happened. This leads to many memory errors for these individuals who are led to believe by their imagination of the event that it is real, has occurred and thus is stored in their memory for that reason.[59]

Consequences of memory errors[edit]

Memory errors often lead to the belief of new memories, which are problematic. In the case of eyewitness testimony new false memories can often lead to wrong information and lack of conviction or wrongful conviction of individuals. Also in the case of child abuse, memory errors can lead to the creation of false traumatic childhood memories, which can lead to false accusations and loss of trust.

Eyewitness testimony[edit]

Memory errors can occur in eyewitness testimonies due to a number of features commonly present in a trial, all of which may influence the authenticity of the memory, and may be detrimental to the outcome of the case at hand.
Such features include:

Leading questions
refers to how wording of questions can influence how an event is remembered. This can result from a misinformation effect or an imagination inflation effect.[60] The misinformation effect occurs when information is presented after the events in question have occurred which leads to memory errors in later retrieval.[61] Studies have suggested that witnesses may misattribute accuracy to misleading information because the sources of misleading information and witnessed information become confused.[61] Misleading information can be acquired through reading of the newspaper, watching the news, being interviewed or when sitting in the courthouse during the trial. When witnesses are asked to recall specific details of an event they can begin to doubt their memory, which can cause memory errors. Misinformation can manifest itself as a memory leading the individual to believe it as true and witnesses may also begin to doubt their own memories of the event deciding instead that they must be wrong.[61] Memory errors also occur through the imagination inflation effect. As stated earlier, the imagination inflation effect takes place when an individual imagines an event to the point where it is believed as a memory of an actual event.[60] During trial, witnesses hear many different possible occurrences of events and are led to imagine these situations. Through imagining and rehearsal of the occurrences, witnesses may begin to see vividness and validity in the stories simply from rehearsal, not factual memories.[60] This can create problems for witnesses when trying to distinguish between imagined events and the actual occurrence of the events. Small but largely significant details become easily mixed and these occurrences of memory errors can make or break a trial.
Weapons Focus Effect
refers to the fact that witnesses are highly likely to pay close attention to the weapon being used during an event, which creates a reduction in the ability to remember other details regarding the crime.[62] This can in turn create memory errors leaving the witness less able to remember details such as what the assailant was wearing or what distinctive features could be found on their body or face. One explanation for why witnesses tend to gravitate toward the weapon being used is said to be that the arousal of the witness is increased.[63] When arousal becomes increased the number of perceptional cues being utilized by the brain decreases.[63] This allows the individual to focus on the weapon cue and ignore other cues such as distinct scars or a bright red shirt. The weapons focus effect can also refer to how the report of the use of weapons in the case can influence the memory of the event, leading to a false memory of having heard a weapon being fired even if the witness did not.[63] For example, if a newspaper reports that the victim was beaten with a hammer, upon reading this, the witness will begin to believe that a hammer was in fact used, even if they at no point saw a hammer. This can cause many memory errors and conflict of stories for witnesses. As a society we believe that newspapers or televised news reports have fact behind them. If they report the hammer being used, a witness might begin to second-guess their memory wondering if they missed the hammer or failed to remember that detail.[62] Also, their story may become mixed with the media’s representation of the story and the knife that they did see will be forgotten and instead be replaced with the hammer that was reported.[63]
Familiarity Effect
refers to the tendency of individuals to develop a preference for things simply because they are familiar.[64] This can leave an individual to identify familiar people as guilty, even if they are not. When we are continuously exposed to the same object or person we begin to develop a positive attraction towards said object or person. Simply their familiarity creates a positive sense when re-exposed to the individual or object. In reality one can know very little about a person but by seeing them over and over again can gain unconscious positive recall for their face. This can create memory errors when individuals are asked to identify a criminal and someone familiar to them is placed in the line up. When a familiar face is in among the individuals that the witness is being asked to study, the witness will find himself or herself gravitating towards the familiar face whether or not this is who they witnessed committing the crime. This leaves them more likely to ignore the cues that are leading them towards other individuals and concentrate on the familiar face, resulting in a false accusation. The sense of familiarity can play a large role in the identification of criminals but when the familiarity of a criminal is mixed in with the familiarity of other individuals, choosing the right person can become quite difficult.

Child abuse[edit]

Memory errors regarding the recovery of repressed childhood abuse can occur due to post-event suggestions from a trusted source, such as a family member, or more commonly, a mental health professional. Due to possible relationships between childhood abuse and mental illness later in life, some mental health professionals believe in the Freudian theory of repressed memories as a defense mechanism for the anxiety that recall of the abuse would cause. Freud said that repression operates unconsciously in individuals who are not able to recall a threatening situation or may even forget that the abusive individual was ever part of their lives. Therefore, mental health professionals will sometimes seek to uncover possible instances of childhood abuse in patients, which may lead to suggestibility and cause a false memory of childhood abuse to arise, in an attempt to seek a cause to a mental illness.[65] No matter the confidence in the memory, this does not necessarily equate to the memory being true. This is an example of the misinformation effect and false memory effect. The fact that memories are not retrieved as whole entities but rather are reconstructed from information remaining in memory and other related knowledge make them easily susceptible to memory errors.[66] This explains why working with mental health professionals and leading questions can sometimes manifest false memories by creating knowledge of possible events and asking individuals to focus on if these events actually took place.[67] Individuals begin to overthink these situations visualizing them in their mind and overanalyzing them. This in turn leads to the belief of situations and vivid memories. Patients are left with memories they believe are real and new events from their childhood which can lead to stress and trauma in their adult life and loss of relationships with those who are believed to be the abuser.

See also[edit]

  • Amnesia
  • False memory syndrome
  • Memory implantation
  • Memory loss
  • Memory and aging
  • Memory bias
  • Memory conformity
  • Kleptomnesia

References[edit]

  1. ^ a b Roediger, H. L., III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814
  2. ^ a b c d e f Schacter, Daniel L. (2011). Psychology Second Edition. New York: Worth Publishers. pp. 246. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
  3. ^ a b c d Schacter, Daniel (2011). Psychology 2nd Ed. New York: Worth Publishers. p. 243.
  4. ^ Schacter, Daniel L.,[«Psychology 2nd Ed.»],»Worth Publishers»,2009,2011.
  5. ^ a b Schacter, Daniel L., Daniel T. Gilbert, and Daniel M. Wegner. «Chapter 6: Memory.» Psychology. ; Second Edition. N.p.: Worth, Incorporated, 2011. 244-45.
  6. ^ Schacter, Daniel L., Daniel T. Gilbert, and Daniel M. Wegner. «Chapter 6: Memory.» Psychology. ; Second Edition. N.p.: Worth, Incorporated, 2011. 245.
  7. ^ a b c d e f Loftus, E. (1997). Creating false memories. Scientific American, 277, 70–75
  8. ^ a b c Johnson, M. & Raye, C. (1998). False memories and confabulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(4), 137–145
  9. ^ a b c d e f g Schacter, Daniel L. (2011). Psychology Second Edition. New York: Worth Publishers. pp. 253–254. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
  10. ^ Loftus, E. F. & Hoffman, H. G. (1989). Misinformation and memory, the creation of new memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 118(1), 100–104
  11. ^ Loftus, E. F. & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585–589
  12. ^ a b Deffenbacher, K.A., Bornstein, B.H., & Penrod, S.D. (2006). Mugshot exposure effects: Retroactive interference, mugshot commitment, source confusion, and unconscious transference. Law and Human Behaviour, 30(3), 287–307
  13. ^ Daniel L. Schacter, «Searching for memory: the brain, the mind, and the past», (New York, 1996)
  14. ^ Mazzoni, G. & Memon, A. (2003). Imagination can create false autobiographical memories. Psychological Science, 14(2), 186–188
  15. ^ Deese, J. (1959). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 17–22
  16. ^ a b c d Kleider, H.M., Pezdek, K., Goldinger, S.D., & Kirk, A. (2008). Schema-driven source misattribution errors: Remembering the expected from a witnessed event. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1–20
  17. ^ Jacobs, D. (1990). Intrusion errors in the figural memory of patients with Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, 49–57.
  18. ^ a b c d Stip, E., Corbière, M., Boulay, L. J., Lesage, A., Lecomte, T., Leclerc, C., Richard, N., Cyr, M., & Guillem, F. (2007). Intrusion errors in explicit memory: Their differential relationship with clinical and social outcome in chronic schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 12(2), 112–127
  19. ^ a b c Kahana et al. (2006). Temporal Associations and Prior list intrusions in Free Recall.
  20. ^ Smith, Troy, A., Kimball, Daniel, R. Kahana, Michael, J. (2007). The fSAM Model of False Recall.
  21. ^ Wickelgren, Wayne, A., (1965. Similarity and Intrusions in Short Term Memory for Consonant-Vowel Digrams.
  22. ^ Wickelgren, Wayne, A., (1965). Acoustic Similarity and Intrusion Errors in Short Term Memory.
  23. ^ a b c d e Hyman Jr., I.E., & Loftus, E.F. (1998). Errors in autobiographical memory. Clinical Psychology Review, 18(8), 933–947
  24. ^ a b Greenberg, D.L. (2004). President Bush’s false «flashbulb» memory of 9/11/01. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 363–370
  25. ^ Laney, C., & Loftus, E.F. (2010). Truth in emotional memories. In B.H. Bornstein (Ed.), Emotion and the Law (pp. 157–183). Leicester: Springer Science + Business Media
  26. ^ a b c Wade, K.A., & Garry, M. (2005). Strategies for verifying false autobiographical memories. American Journal of Psychology, 118(4), 587–602
  27. ^ Mojet J, Ko¨ster EP. Sensory memory and food texture. Food Qual Prefer 2005; 16: 251–266. doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2004.04.017.
  28. ^ a b Dell, G.S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93(3), 283–321
  29. ^ Anderson, J.R. (1983). A spreading activation theory of memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 261–295
  30. ^ a b Kapatsinski, V. (2004). Frequency, age-of-acquisition, lexicon size, neighborhood density, and speed of processing: towards a domain-general, single mechanism account. High Desert Linguistic Society Conference, 6, 131–150.
  31. ^ a b Forster, K.I. (1991). The density constraint on form-priming in the naming task: Interference effects from a masked prime. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(1), 1–25.
  32. ^ a b c Jaimes, A., Omura, K., Nagamine, T., & Hirata, K. (2004). Memory cues for meeting video retrieval. In
    Proceedings of the 1st ACM Workshop on Continuous Archival and Retrieval of Personal Experiences
    (CARPE) at the ACM International Multimedia Conference
  33. ^ Tulving, E., & Thomson, D.M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352–373
  34. ^ Smith, S.M. (1984). A comparison of two techniques for reducing context-dependent forgetting. Memory and Cognition, 12(5), 477–482.
  35. ^ Eich, J.E. (1980). The cue-dependent nature of state-dependent retrieval. Memory and Cognition, 8(2), 157–173.
  36. ^ Thomson, D.M., & Tulving, E. (1970). Associative encoding and retrieval: Weak and strong cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86(2), 255–262.
  37. ^ a b Moscovitc, M., & Craik, F.I.M. (1976). Depth of processing, retrieval cues, and uniqueness of encoding as factors in recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 15, 447–458.
  38. ^ Baddeley, A.D., & Dale, H.C.A. (1966). The effect of semantic similarity on retro-active interference in long- and short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 417–420.
  39. ^ Badre, D. & Wagner, A.D. (2005). Frontal lobe mechanisms that resolve proactive interference. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 2003–2012.
  40. ^ Dewar, M.T., Cowan, N., & Della Sala, S. (2007). Forgetting due to retroactive interference: A fusion of Muller and Pilzecker’s (1900) early insights into everyday forgetting and recent research on anterograde amnesia. Cortex, 43(5), 616–634.
  41. ^ Conway, M. A., & Tacchi, P. C. (1996). Motivated confabulation. Neurocase, 2, 325–338
  42. ^ Treves, A. & Rolls, E. T. (1994). Computational analysis of the role of the hippocampus in memory. Neuroscience, 4(3), 374–391
  43. ^ Kensinger, E.A., & Corkin, S. (2004). The effects of emotional content and aging on false memories. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioural Neuroscience, 4(1), 1–9.
  44. ^ a b c d e f g h Schacter, D.L., Koutstaal, W., & Norman, K.A. (1997). False memories and aging. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(6).
  45. ^ Kensinger, E.A. (2009). Remembering the details: Effects of emotion. Emotion Review, 1(2), 99–113
  46. ^ Dolcos, F., & Denkova, E. (2008). Neural correlates of encoding emotional memories: A review of functional neuroimaging evidence. Cell Science, 5(2)
  47. ^ Schmidt, S.R. (2004). Autobiographical memories for the September 11 attacks: Reconstructive errors and emotional impairment of memory. Memory and Cognition, 32(3), 443–454
  48. ^ Ruci, L., Tomes, J.L., & Zelenski, J.M. (2009). Mood-congruent false memories in the DRM paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 23(6), 1153–1165
  49. ^ Baddeley, A.D., Bressi, S., Della Sala, S., Logie, R., & Spinnler, H. (1991). The decline of working memory in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 114, 2521–2542.
  50. ^ Mimura, M., & Komatsu, S.I. (2010). Factors of error and effort in memory intervention for patients with Alzheimer’s disease and amnesic syndrome. Psychogeriatrics, 10, 179–186.
  51. ^ a b Mitchell, J.P., Sullivan, A.L., Schacter, D.L., & Budson, A.E. (2006). Misattribution errors in Alzheimer’s disease: The illusory truth effect. Neuropsychology, 20(2), 185–192.
  52. ^ Hildebrandt, H., Haldenwanger, A., & Eling, P. (2009). False recognition helps to distinguish patients with Alzheimer’s disease and amnesic mci from patients with other kinds of dementia. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 28(2).
  53. ^ a b Kramer, J.H., Delis, D.C., Blusewicz, M.J., & Brandt, J. (1988). Verbal memory errors in Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s dementias. Developmental Neuropsychology, 4(1), 1–15.
  54. ^ Kern, B.S., Gorp, W.G.V., Cummings, J.L., Brown, W.S., & Osato, S.S. (1992). Confabulation in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 19(2), 172–182.
  55. ^ Fuld, P.A., Katzman, R., Davies, P., & Terry, R.D. (2004). Intrusions as a sign of Alzheimer dementia chemical and pathological verification. Annals of Neurology, 11(2), 155–159.
  56. ^ a b c d e Ilsley, J.E., Moffoot, A.P.R., & O’Carroll, R.E. (1995). An analysis of memory dysfunction in major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 35, 1–9.
  57. ^ a b c d Fridberg, D.J., Brenner, A., & Lysaker, P.H. (2010). Verbal memory intrusions in schizophrenia: Associations with self-reflectivity, symptomatology, and neurocognition. Psychiatry Research, 179, 6–11.
  58. ^ Brébion, G., Amador, X., Smith, M.J., Malaspina, D., Sharif, Z. & Gorman, J.M. (1999). Opposite links of positive and negative symptomatology with memory errors in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 88, 15–24.
  59. ^ a b c Mammarella, N., Altamura M., Padalino F.A., Petito A., Fairfield B. & Bellomo A. (2010). False memories in schizophrenia? An imagination inflation study. Psychiatry Research, 179, 267–273.
  60. ^ a b c Whitehouse, W.G., Orne, E.C., Dinges, D.F. (2010). Extreme cognitive interviewing: A blueprint for false memories through imagination inflation. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 58(3), 269–287
  61. ^ a b c English, S.M., Nielson, K.A. (2010). Reduction of the misinformation effect by arousal induced after learning. Cognition, 117(2), 237–242.
  62. ^ a b Loftus, E.F., Loftus, G.R., Messo, J. (1987). Some fact about “weapon focus”. Law and Human Behavior, 11(1), 55–62.
  63. ^ a b c d Mitchell, K.J., Livosky, M., Mather, M. (1998). The weapon focus effect revisited: The role of novelty. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 287–303
  64. ^ Moreland, R.L., Zajonc, R.B. (1982). Exposure effects in person perception: Familiarity, similarity, and attraction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 18(5), 395–415
  65. ^ Lindsay, D.S., & Read, J.D. (1994). Psychotherapy and memories of childhood sexual abuse: A cognitive perspective. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 281–338.
  66. ^ Hyman Jr., I.E., & Pentland, J. (1996). The role of mental imagery in the creation of false childhood memories. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 101–117.
  67. ^ Lindsay, D.S., & Read, J.D. (1995). Memory work and recovered memories of childhood sexual abuse: Scientific evidence and public, professional, and personal issues. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 1(4), 846–908

Как исправить синий экран MEMORY_MANAGEMENTСиний экран MEMORY_MANAGEMENT в Windows 10 — распространенная, и, одновременно, часто сложная в диагностике проблема, однако, выяснить её причины и устранить обычно оказывается возможным.

В этой инструкции подробно о возможных методах исправить ошибку MEMORY MANAGEMENT в Windows 10 и о том, чем она может быть вызвана.

  • Простые способы исправить ошибку MEMORY MANAGEMENT
  • Синий экран MEMORY MANAGEMENT — поиск причины и исправление
  • Что делать, если ошибка возникает до загрузки Windows 10

Простые способы исправить ошибку MEMORY MANAGEMENT

Ошибка MEMORY_MANAGEMENT в Windows 10

В связи со сложностью определения причин синего экрана MEMORY MANAGEMENT начинающими пользователями, рекомендую начать с простых подходов, которые, возможно, смогут достичь решения проблемы, не прибегая к более сложным манипуляциям.

Эти методы подойдут в случае, если Windows 10 запускается и какое-то время работает, а синий экран появляется позже:

  1. Если ошибка возникла после недавних обновлений системы или драйверов, возможно, использования каких-либо программ для оптимизации и ускорения Windows 10, попробуйте использовать точки восстановления системы (Панель управления — Запуск восстановления системы).
  2. Если точек восстановления нет, а вы обновляли какие-то драйверы, попробуйте зайти в диспетчер устройств (правый клик по кнопке «Пуск» — «Диспетчер устройств»), выбрать устройство, драйвер которого обновлялся, открыть его свойства и на вкладке «Драйвер» проверить доступность кнопки «Откатить».
  3. Если вы точно знаете, после установки какой программы появилась проблема, а программа работает в фоновом режиме (автоматически загружаясь с системой), возможно, стоит удалить эту программу или убрать её из автозагрузки Windows 10.
  4. Если устанавливались какие-либо сторонние антивирусы, они могут быть причиной ошибки. Проверьте, будет ли она проявлять себя, если отключить или удалить этот антивирус.
  5. Если синий экран MEMORY MANAGEMENT стал появляться сразу после чистой установки Windows 10 на компьютер или ноутбук, возможно, причина в том, что те драйверы, которые установила сама система не вполне подходят. Вручную скачайте и установите официальные драйверы с сайта производителя ноутбук или материнской платы (если это ПК) — включая драйверы чипсета и другие драйверы.
  6. Если вы включали какие-то опции ускорения памяти, процессора, видеокарты в БИОС/UEFI или с помощью сторонних программ, попробуйте отключить их.
  7. Если ошибка MEMORY_MANAGEMENT появляется только при запуске определенной игры или программы, проблема может быть именно в этой программе или, если игра или программа платная, а вы её не совсем купили, то в антивирусе (попробовать запуск при отключенном антивирусе, в том числе встроенном Защитнике Windows).

К сожалению, не всегда указанные простые подходы к решению проблемы оказываются результативными. В этом случае переходим к более сложным вариантам определения причин проблемы и исправления ошибки.

Синий экран MEMORY_MANAGEMENT — ищем причину и исправляем ошибку

Следующие этапы могут помочь разобраться в том, что именно вызывает появление рассматриваемого синего экрана. Опять же, предполагается, что вы можете зайти в Windows 10 и работать в ней. Если же синий экран MEMORY MANAGEMENT появляется до загрузки системы — об этом в последнем разделе инструкции.

Чистая загрузка Windows 10

В качестве первого шага следует попробовать выполнить чистую загрузку Windows 10 без сторонних служб и программ в автозагрузке — это позволит выяснить, не являются ли какие-либо сторонние программы причиной ошибки. Для того, чтобы сделать это, выполните следующие шаги:

  1. Нажмите клавиши Win+R на клавиатуре, введите msconfig в окно «Выполнить» и нажмите Enter. Запуск msconfig в Windows 10
  2. На вкладке «Общие» отметьте пункт «Выборочный запуск» и снимите отметку «Загружать элементы автозагрузки».
  3. Перейдите на вкладку «Службы», отметьте «Не отображать службы Майкрософт», а затем нажмите «Отключить все». Настройка чистой загрузки Windows 10
  4. Желательно также зайти на вкладку «Автозагрузка» перейти в параметры автозагрузки диспетчера задач и там отключить все пункты (даже нужные).
  5. Примените настройки и перезагрузите компьютер.

Если после перезагрузки ошибка MEMORY_MANAGEMENT больше не появляется, по-видимому, причина в одной из программ в автозагрузке или какой-то сторонней службе. Можно попробовать включать элементы по одному или несколько, пока снова не появится проблема, чтобы выяснить, что именно её вызвало и после этого принимать решения о возможном удалении этого стороннего ПО или других действиях.

Проверка драйверов

Синий экран MEMORY MANAGEMENT часто может быть вызван проблемой с драйверами. Для того, чтобы выявить сбойный драйвер оборудования, можно использовать встроенную утилиту для проверки драйверов:

  1. Перезагрузите компьютер в безопасном режиме. Самый простой метод сделать это — нажать клавиши Win+R на клавиатуре, ввести msconfig и нажать Enter, отметить «Безопасный режим» на вкладке «Загрузка», применить настройки и перезагрузить компьютер.
  2. В безопасном режиме нажмите клавиши Win+R и введите verifier для запуска диспетчера проверки драйверов.
  3. На первом экране выберите «Создать нестандартные параметры (для кода программ)». Настройка диспетчера проверки драйверов
  4. Далее отметьте все пункты, кроме «Эмуляция случайной нехватки ресурсов» и двух пунктов, связанных с проверкой соответствия требованиям DDI. Пункты проверки драйверов
  5. Выберите пункт «Выбрать имя драйвера из списка». Выбрать файлы драйверов из списка
  6. Отсортируйте драйверы по колонке «Поставщик» (нажав на заголовок столбца) и отметьте все драйверы, кроме предоставленных Microsoft. Нажмите кнопку «Готово». Выбор драйверов в Driver Verifier
  7. Если вы запускали безопасный режим с помощью msconfig, снова зайдите (как на первом шаге) и отключите загрузку в безопасном режиме.
  8. Перезагрузите компьютер.

После этого возможны следующие варианты развития событий:

  • Система загрузится, некоторое время проработает, а затем появится синий экран с прежней ошибкой MEMORY_MANAGEMENT или новой, наподобие DRIVER_VERIFIED_DETECTED_VIOLATION с указанием имени файла .sys (если имя файла указано — запомните или запишите его, это и есть сбойный драйвер).
  • Система покажет синий экран еще до загрузки системы.

В первом случае перезагрузите компьютер и отключите проверку драйверов, для этого запустите командную строку от имени администратора и введите команду verifier /reset

После этого, если на синем экране отображалось имя сбойного драйвера, найдите в Интернете, что это за драйвер — по всей видимости, проблема именно в нем. Если имя драйвера не отображалось, попробуйте использовать бесплатную утилиту BlueScreenView и в ней посмотреть, какой именно файл вызвал последний сбой (на скриншоте пример для другого синего экрана). Утилита доступна на официальном сайте https://www.nirsoft.net/utils/blue_screen_view.html

Анализ ошибки MEMORY MANAGEMENT в BlueScreenView

Если система не запускается, то после двух сбоев загрузки вы получите возможность открыть дополнительные параметры и инструменты восстановления и там, запустив командную строку, отключить проверку драйверов с помощью команды verifier /reset а затем перезагрузить компьютер. Далее, можно использовать действия из предыдущего абзаца.

После выявления вызывающего неполадки драйвера вы можете предпринять действия по его обновлению (в идеале — путем скачивания вручную с официального сайта производителя оборудования) или откату на предыдущую версию.

Проверка оперативной памяти и целостности системных файлов, ошибок диска

Иногда ошибка может быть вызвана проблемами с оперативной памятью. Если у вас есть подозрения на то, что дело в этом, то для начала рекомендую отключить любые ускорения RAM, если раньше вы их применяли, а затем попробовать выполнить проверку оперативной памяти на ошибки.

Еще пара рекомендаций — выполнить проверку целостности системных файлов Windows 10 и проверку жесткого диска на ошибки.

Исправление ошибки, если система не запускается

Дополнительные параметры восстановления Windows 10

В случае, если сообщение об ошибке MEMORY MANAGEMENT появляется еще до загрузки системы, то после двух неудачных загрузок вы должны получить доступ в дополнительные параметры восстановления в разделе «Поиск и устранение неисправностей», где вы можете:

  1. Попробовать загрузку в безопасном режиме (пункт «Параметры загрузки»).
  2. Выполнить восстановление из точек восстановления (пункт «Восстановление системы»).
  3. Удалить последние установленные обновления Windows

И в крайнем случае, можно прибегнуть к сбросу системы в исходное состояние.

С синим экраном смерти рано или поздно сталкивается большинство пользователей Windows, и порой даже опытные юзеры не знают, как реагировать на такое поведение компьютера или ноутбука. Сегодня мы поговорим о конкретной разновидности ошибки, вызывающей отказ в работе ПК. Это ошибка «Memory Management», которая появляется чаще всего и кодируется комбинацией 0x0000001A.

Как исправить ошибку Memory Management на Windows

Отметим, что вид синего экрана может различаться в зависимости от версии операционной системы, но содержимое в части информирования пользователя об ошибке одинаковое, что позволяет с довольно грубой погрешностью классифицировать неисправность и приступить к её уточнению и исправлению.

Что означает ошибка «Memory Management» в Windows 10

Данное словосочетание переводится как «Управление памятью», то есть можно предполагать, что произошёл сбой в подсистеме памяти компьютера на аппаратном, программном или операционном уровнях.

Чтобы уточнить диагноз, возможно, придётся осуществлять проверки на всех трёх указанных уровнях.

Ошибка «Memory Management»

При возникновении любой BsoD происходит формирование дампа памяти в момент сбоя, после чего компьютер перегружается, если в настройках не выставлен запрет на перезагрузку в подобных случаях. Если это случайная неполадка, пользователь может и не успеть прочитать надпись. Неслучайный сбой может привести к циклическим перезагрузкам. Чтобы избежать подобного сценария, эксперты рекомендуют выставить опцию запрета на перезагрузку.

Отметим, что причин появления синего экрана смерти с такой надписью существует немало, поэтому и способов поиска неисправности и её устранения тоже имеется большое количество. Обычно сбой происходит во время обмена данными между приложением, запущенным пользователем, и ядром ОС, чаще всего на уровне драйверов.

Ядро системы Windows

Например, уточнение ошибки в Windows 10 в виде надписи «video memory management internal» говорит о том, что проблема возникла в видеоподсистеме компьютера, и для её решения необходимо попробовать переустановить драйвер видеокарты, в редких случаях замены требует драйвер монитора. Обычно это происходит при смене карты или монитора.

Причины появления кода ошибки «Memory Management»

Ядро Windows может конфликтовать с драйверами устройств и/или пользовательскими приложениями по следующим причинам:

  • из-за повреждения драйвера или при установке новой версии, содержащей критические ошибки, проявляющиеся в определённых ситуациях;
  • повреждения могут появиться в самой оперативной памяти (например, ослабление контактов планки с гнездом);
  • фрагментарно может повредиться диск или важные системные файлы;
  • «слетели» настройки BIOS или операционной системы по части электропитания.

Описание ошибки «Memory Management»

И это далеко не полный перечень причин возникновения BsoD.

Методы устранения ошибки

Существует несколько популярных решений, позволяющих решить возникшую проблему без наличия навыков в данной сфере. Рассмотрим их в рамках небольшого обзора.

Отмена последних изменений

Это самый простой и весьма эффективный способ избавиться от проблем, возникших не на аппаратном уровне. Возможно, сбой произошёл после того, как вы установили новое устройство с драйвером, или инсталлировали понравившуюся программу.

Отмена последних изменений

«Правильное» удаление таких программ (через панель управления) вполне может устранить проблему. Если после этого компьютер работает стабильно, поищите альтернативные версии удалённого программного обеспечения, если оно для вас критически важно.

Удаление драйверов

Вторым источником возникновения синего экрана смерти в Windows 10, как мы уже отмечали, является другой тип ПО – драйвера устройств. Обычно, если вы устанавливаете новое оборудование или меняете старое (например, сломавшуюся сетевую карту или морально устаревший графический видеоадаптер), драйвер для нового устройства устанавливается либо автоматически, либо самим пользователем. Найденный драйвер может оказаться не полностью совместимым с установленным оборудованием, и в этом случае возникают конфликты на уровне ядра Виндовс, которые в итоге выливаются с крах системы.

Удаление драйверов

Бывает, что установка нового устройства не привела к смене драйвера, и это тоже вполне вероятная причина сбоя из-за несовместимости драйверного ПО.

Что делать в подобных случаях, понятно: нужно просто удалить неподходящий драйвер. Но без него устройство работать не будет, так что придётся заняться поисками подходящего ПО. Проверенные работоспособные драйверы часто можно скачать на сайтах изготовителей компьютерного оборудования или на специализированных форумах, где хранятся управляющие программы, в том числе для давно не выпускаемого оборудования.

Драйвер можно удалить по-разному, один из самых популярных способов – через «Диспетчер устройств». Он запускается из вкладки «Пуск» или через «Мой компьютер». В перечне присутствующих в системе устройств найдите проблемное, наведите на него курсор и нажмите правой кнопкой мышки. В появившемся окошке нажмите на пункт «Свойства», а в новой вкладке – на надпись «Драйвер». Завершающее действие – нажатие кнопки «Удалить».

Возврат заводских настроек

Нередко проблемы возникают после изменения определённых настроек в BIOS. Это могут быть как опции, связанные с управлением памятью, так и попытка разгона процессора. Хотя современные CPU допускают увеличение частоты по сравнению со штатной, в результате разгона работа системы становится нестабильной. Вот тогда и выскакивает ошибка Виндовс с синим экраном и надписью «Video Memory Management Internal».

Восстановление заводских настроек

Чтобы решить проблему, следует вернуть настройки BIOS к заводским. Практически все микропрограммы БИОС от ведущих производителей материнских плат допускают такую возможность. Хотя интерфейс у них разный, откат на заводские установки можно легко найти по наличию слова Default.

Новое аппаратное обеспечение

Установка в настольный компьютер или ноутбук нового оборудования (необязательно внутреннего) также становится частой причиной возникновения синего экрана с перезагрузкой. Часто ошибка 0x0000001A возникает при установке платы памяти (взамен старой или с целью расширения доступного пула оперативки). Если сбои начали появляться вскоре после такого апгрейда, стоит протестировать новую память на другом компьютере или поработать со старой. Отсутствие проблем в последнем случае будет гарантированно свидетельствовать о бракованной планке, которую придётся менять. Если же сбои продолжатся, нужно искать иную причину.

Такой принцип применим практически ко всему железу.

Но бывает и так, что аппаратная часть оказывается исправной, а причина краха системы заключается в некорректно установленном или содержащем ошибки драйвере. Как её исправить, мы уже рассматривали.

Редко, но случается, что причина кроется в исправном, но некорректно работающем оборудовании. Тут выход только один – обратиться в сервисный центр, где смогут провести полноценную диагностику устройства.

Проверка оперативной памяти

Если вы давненько не устанавливали новое программное обеспечение, то возникновение ошибки «Memory Management» в Windows 10, 8, 7 или XP может быть связано со сбоями в работе оперативной памяти. Собственно, само название ошибки косвенно на это и указывает. Такое случается, если одна из планок начала работать нестабильно, со сбоями. Существует немало методов диагностирования проблемы. Для начала не помешает провести визуальный осмотр, чтобы убедиться в отсутствии видимых дефектов. Если таковых не замечено, попробуйте вынуть планки из слота и протереть контакты спиртовым раствором (не слишком концентрированным) или обычной резинкой для карандашей.

Проверка оперативной памяти

Если сбои будут продолжаться и после этого, следующий шаг – программная проверка памяти на наличие сбойных участков. Такие системные утилиты имеются в составе Виндовс, но более точные результаты демонстрирует ПО от сторонних разработчиков. Одна из лучших таких программ – MemTest86. Цифры в названии пускай вас не смущают – это не год выпуска, а применимость к определённой архитектуре, в данном случае – x86.

Алгоритм поиска ошибок RAM системными средствами:

  • одновременно зажимаем клавиши Win+S;
  • в появившейся строке набираем «Диагностика проблем оперативной памяти компьютера»;
  • выбираем первый вариант из двух появившихся;
  • компьютер перезагрузится, после чего появится надпись «Средство проверки памяти Windows»;
  • как только проверка завершится, произойдёт повторная перезагрузка, после которой и высветятся результаты проверки.

Проведение восстановления системы

Многие пропускают все вышеописанные шаги, начиная именно с этого. В целом восстановление системы действительно решает проблему, если таковая вызвана проблемами софта. Следует учесть, что подобный шаг не всегда оправдан, поскольку при откате к предыдущим состояниям все позже инсталлированные программы исчезнут.

Восстановление Windows

Кроме того, данная опция может быть отключена в настройках (в Windows XP она отключена по умолчанию).

Для запуска функции после нажатия Win+S наберите «Восстановление системы» и выберите соответствующую утилиту в открывшемся списке. Далее следуйте инструкциям.

Восстановление системы Windows

СОВЕТ. Хотя это обратимая операция, выбирайте контрольную точку, не слишком удалённую от текущей даты. Так вы минимизируете возможные потери данных.

Переустановка Windows

Самый кардинальный способ, к которому прибегают, если ничего из перечисленного не помогло и имеется уверенность, что дело не в железе. Обычно на свежеустановленной ОС синий экран смерти не появляется.

Содержание

  1. Ошибка приложения память не может быть read windows
  2. «Ошибка: память не может быть read» Исправляем
  3. Если отключение службы не помогло исправить ошибку приложения память не может быть read?
  4. Системная библиотека ole.dll
  5. Конфликт между программами
  6. Исправление ошибки Memory Management в Windows 10
  7. Что собой представляет ошибка?
  8. Почему появляется проблема?
  9. Алгоритмы решения конфликта
  10. Было установлено новое программное обеспечение
  11. Повреждения системных файлов или диска
  12. Обнаружение конфликтного драйвера
  13. Другие проблемы
  14. How To Fix Windows Stop Code Memory Management BSOD
  15. What Causes the Windows Stop Stop Code Memory Management BSOD?
  16. Run the Windows Memory Diagnostic Tool
  17. Check Your Drivers and Update Windows
  18. Check for Corrupted System Files
  19. Reset Windows 10
  20. Replace Your Hardware
  21. Fixing the Windows Stop Code Memory Management BSOD Error

Ошибка приложения память не может быть read windows

Ошибка память не может быть read – не такое частое явление у современных компьютеров. Отчасти поэтому, когда на экране вдруг появляется сообщение «Ошибка: память не может быть read», пользователи, буквально, впадают в ступор.

Есть несколько принципиальных моментов.

  1. Ошибки памяти связаны с другими ошибками – устройств или приложений.
  2. В большинстве случаев, когда «память не может быть read», Windows предусматривает алгоритм действий для исправления ситуации и предотвращения ее повторений.
  3. Если все делать правильно, то, вопреки распространенному мнению, чаще всего, удается все исправить без переустановки операционной системы.
  4. Сама ситуация, когда память не может быть прочитана, означает, что некое приложение попыталось получить доступ к неким страницам памяти, доступ к которым ему запрещен. Затем Windows, «узнав» об этом, блокирует само приложение, а пользователю вывешивает искомое сообщение об ошибке.

«Ошибка: память не может быть read» Исправляем

Отключение DEP (Службы прекращения данных). Эта служба как раз контролирует использование памяти программами. В частности, примером ее работы служит сообщение о прекращении работы приложения, которое может появиться на экране с заголовком «Прекращена работа программы». Программа закрывается, результат работы теряется, поскольку память блокируется.

Чтобы добраться до службы. Идем в Панель управления – Система. Слева находим ссылку Дополнительные параметры, в окошке переходим на вкладочку Дополнительно, затем Быстродействие и Параметры.

Нам нужна вкладка Предотвращение выполнения данных. Включаем переключатель, позволяющий активировать службу DEP для отдельных программ. Теперь добавляем в список те программы-исключения, для которых хотим службу отключить, с помощью кнопки «Добавить».

Остается только дважды щелкнуть мышью исполняемый файл выбранной программы и нажать «Ок». После перезагрузки изменения вступят в силу.

Это было редактирование работы службы. Теперь рассмотрим, как ее отключить.

  • Запускаем командную строку: Пуск, в строке поиска ввести «cmd» и нажать Enter. Программу, когда система ее найдет, запустить от имени администратора с помощью меню правой кнопки мыши.
  • В окне программы нужно будет ввести следующую строку (внимательно, без ошибок!): bcdedit.exe/set nx AlwaysOff
  • Жмем Enter, перезагружаемся.
  • Если понадобится снова включить службу, нужно будет проделать все то же самое, только ввести команду: bcdedit.exe/set nx OptIn

Если отключение службы не помогло исправить ошибку приложения память не может быть read?

Значит ваша «Ошибка приложения: память не может быть read» имеет несколько иное происхождение.

Системная библиотека ole.dll

Если установка/удаление программ по каким-то причинам происходило некорректно, то это отражается на содержимом системной библиотеки. Ошибки копятся и имеют, в том числе, и такое проявление. Чтобы их исправить, нужно в строке «Выполнить» (Поиск) меню Пуск ввести команду: regsvr32 %SystemRoot%system32ole32.dll.
После этого нажать Enter и перезагрузиться.

Конфликт между программами

Такое тоже бывает в этой жизни: одна программа создает проблемы для другой и вместе они работать никак не хотят. Те, кто давно работает с компьютерами, наверняка помнят, как некогда намертво зависала система, когда одновременно устанавливали антивирус (Касперского или ESET) и брандмауэр McAfee Firewall. Отличный пример подобной несовместимости.

Чтобы избежать подобного. Следует просто следовать нескольким нехитрым правилам.
Соблюдать ну хоть какую-то упорядоченность и разборчивость в устанавливаемых приложениях (проще говоря, не устанавливать все подряд).

Регулярно производить очистку дисков и системы. Желательно полную и бескомпромиссную. С привлечением «спецсредств», хотя бы простого и всем знакомого CCleaner или других подобных приложений.

Таковы основные причины, приводящие к ошибкам чтения памяти. Конечно, это не полный список, однако, если воспользоваться нашими советами, возможно, вы уже никогда не увидите неприятных сообщений о невозможности чтения памяти.

Исправление ошибки Memory Management в Windows 10

Пользователи, установившие последнюю операционную систему от Microsoft, регулярно сталкиваются с недоработками ее создателей. Сегодня узнаем, каким образом устраняется ошибка memory management в Windows 10.

Что собой представляет ошибка?

Неполадка относится к так называемым BsoD, появление которых сопровождается созданием дампа памяти и перезагрузкой компьютера. Источников данной проблемы масса, столько же и решений, для каждого случая оно будет собственным.

Отметим, что надпись «video memory management internal» в окне ошибки касается неполадок в видеосистеме: следует переустановить драйвер видеокарты, заменив его на новый или более старый, но стабильный. Реже источником ошибки становится драйвер монитора или мониторов.

Почему появляется проблема?

Синий экран смерти вызывается целым рядом факторов, которые производят к конфликту ядра Windows 10 с каким-либо приложением/драйвером.

  • Драйвер может быть повреждённым или в его новой версии есть критические ошибки.
  • Оперативная память физически повреждена или имеет проблемы с контактами.
  • Повреждены системные файлы или диск.
  • В BIOS или Windows 10 необходимо правильно настроить схему электропитания.

Алгоритмы решения конфликта

Как исправить ошибку, если ее вызывает множество факторов? Начинать, пожалуй, следует из выявления ее источника и самых простых вариантов его устранения, которые не отнимут у пользователя много времени, а лишь потом браться за более сложные методы исправления неполадки.

Было установлено новое программное обеспечение

Если BSoD начал появляться после инсталляции или обновления какой-либо программы, драйвера или апгрейда Windows 10, необходимо все вернуть на свои места, то есть удалить последние обновления, откатить драйвер или программу до последней версии. Но прежде всего, следует выяснить виновника ситуации. Сделать это поможет чистый запуск Windows 10.

2. В текстовой строке пишем «msconfig» и нажимаем «OK» или «Ввод».

3. На первой вкладке переносим чекбокс к позиции «Выборочный запуск».

5. Посещаем вкладку «Службы», отключаем отображение сервисов от Microsoft и все остальные службы.

6. Идем во вкладку «Автозагрузка», запускаем «Диспетчер задач» и в нем в одноименной вкладке деактивируем все приложения.

7. Теперь перезагружаем компьютер и делаем все то, что вызывало появление ошибки.

Если она не возникла, вероятнее всего, ее появление связано с отключенной службой, драйвером или приложением. Здесь необходимо по порядку включать каждый отключенный элемент, если не уверены в виновнике проблемы, и выявить его. После удалить, обновить или откатить до предыдущей версии, в зависимости от ситуации.

Здесь же будет очень полезной функция отката, возврата состояния системы к одной из ранее созданных точек восстановления, если она включена. Выбирать следует последнюю точку перед появлением неполадки.

Повреждения системных файлов или диска

Для проверки контрольных сумм системных файлов и их восстановления следует воспользоваться запущенной от имени администратора командной строкой.

Вводим и выполняем команду «sfc /scannow».

Также следует проверить поверхность системного диска на присутствие в нем битых секторов. Выполняем «chkdsk C: /F /R» в этом же окне.

Если Windows 10 не позволяет запустить командную строку, к примеру, ошибка появляется на этапе или сразу после запуска ОС, загрузитесь с установочного дистрибутива Виндовс той же редакции, и в окне с кнопкой «Установить» запускайте нужный инструмент.

Обнаружение конфликтного драйвера

1. Запускаемся в безопасном режиме, например, через «Параметры».

2. Выполняем команду «verifier» в окне, запускаемом через Win+R.

3. В «Диспетчере проверки» создаем нестандартный параметр.

4. Указываем отображённые на скриншоте параметры из приведенного в окне списка, и ставим флажок возле опции «Прочие проверки».

5. Ставим триггерный переключатель в последнее положение: «Выбирать имя драйвера из списка».

6. Сортируем все продукты по поставщику и снимаем флажки с драйверов от Microsoft.

7. Перезагружаемся для запуска анализа и сбора информации.

Здесь при обнаружении проблемы Windows 10 может выдать синий экран, перезагрузиться и зациклиться на этом. Но паниковать не следует, разработчики предусмотрели подобные случаи: после двух перезагрузок появится меню диагностики с предложениями выбрать путь решения проблемы.

8. В дополнительных параметрах выбираем вариант реанимации системы во время запуска.

9. Перезагружаемся в безопасном режиме с возможностью вызова командной строки.

9. В ней выполняем перечень команд:

  1. verifier /reset – для деактивации проверки драйверов;
  2. shutdown -r -t 0 – стандартная перезагрузка ПК.

10. Через проводник или файловый менеджер переходим в каталог «windowsminidump» на системном диске.

11. При помощи свободно распространяемой утилиты BlueScreenView открываем дамп памяти с целью выполнить анализ результатов тестирования и проверки драйверов.

12. Изучаем информацию, содержащуюся в подсвеченных розовым цветом строках в нижнем фрейме.

13. По названию проблемных файлов или библиотек определяем, к каким программам они относятся и делаем откат драйверов этих устройств, удаляем и устанавливаем заново или обновляем. В разных ситуациях помогают различные решения.

Здесь на помощь придет не только логическое мышление и знания (понятно, что при наличии букв dx драйвер относится к платформе DirectX, nv – касается видеокарты Nvidia и т. д.), но и поисковые системы. По названию файла найти драйвер, в состав коего он входит, несложно.

Загружать драйверы следует вручную и только с официальных ресурсов поддержки устройств. Обновлять их через Центр обновления ни в коем случаи не следует.

Другие проблемы

Приведем еще несколько рекомендаций, которые периодически помогают вернуть Windows 10 к штатному режиму функционирования.

1. Проверка ОЗУ при помощи синтетического теста, например, Memtest86.

2. Обновление BIOS до последней версии (это следует выполнять осторожно или обратиться в сервисный центр).

3. Установите сбалансированную схему питания на портативных устройствах при работе от сети.

Не лишней будет очистка реестра, системного тома от мусора и сканирование ПК на вирусы.

How To Fix Windows Stop Code Memory Management BSOD

A rare problem with several fixes to try

Unlike older versions of Windows, Windows 10 is remarkably stable. Long gone are the old Blue Screen of Death (BSOD) errors, but that doesn’t mean they’re gone for good. Windows will still throw a BSOD error your way when it detects problems, such as a Windows stop code memory management error.

This kind of error is very specific, and despite the complex-sounding name, BSOD errors like these can be easy to troubleshoot and resolve—in most cases. The Windows stop code memory management BSOD hints at a problem with your system memory, so here are a few fixes you can try to resolve the issue.

What Causes the Windows Stop Stop Code Memory Management BSOD?

Memory management is, as you might guess from the name, an important part of your Windows installation—it keeps your RAM usage steady while you’re using your PC. When this process fails, and rogue processes soak up all of your available memory, your system becomes more prone to crashes and failures.

This BSOD error is one of the inevitable results of that. There are plenty of reasons why a memory management error can occur, including outdated software and drivers, corrupt files, and hardware failure (especially with your RAM or hard drive).

In the first instance, restart your PC and see if that fixes the problem. If it doesn’t, you’ll need to take the following steps.

As an error related to system memory, you should give the built-in Windows Memory Diagnostic Tool a try first. This will run a quick check of your system memory for any obvious errors, with the ability to run a boot-level check immediately or postpone the check until the next time you reboot your PC.

  • To run this tool, press Windows key + R on your keyboard to open the Run launch box, type in MdSched, then press OK or hit the Enter key to launch it.

  • The Windows Memory Diagnostic window will open. Click Restart now and check for problems (Recommended) to immediately run a boot-level check of your RAM, or click Check for problems the next time I start my button to schedule the check when your PC next restarts.

Depending on the option you select, the Windows Memory Diagnostic Tool will run its check immediately or on your next reboot. The tool will take some time to complete its check, depending on your system.

When the test is complete, you’ll need to open the Event Viewer to view the log report.

  • To do this, right-click the Windows start menu and press Event Viewer. Once it opens, click Event Viewer (Local) > Windows Logs > System, click Find, type MemoryDiagnostic, then press Find Next to begin searching for the report.

  • The first result it finds will be the result of your test—any errors it finds will be shown under the General tab at the bottom for you to troubleshoot further.

If no errors are detected, but the BSOD error continues to occur, you’ll need to investigate other problem fixes.

Check Your Drivers and Update Windows

While it isn’t a miracle fix, an easy stop code memory management Windows 10 fix is also an obvious one—update your drivers, and check for any available Windows system updates to install essential bug fixes and device optimizations.

You can search for Windows 10 updates (including driver updates) from the Windows Settings menu.

  • Right-click the Start menu and click Settings to open the Windows Settings menu, then press Update & Security. Click Download or Download and Install for any available updates listed.

For certain devices (such as your graphics card), you may need to visit the manufacturer’s website to download the latest, up-to-date drivers.

Check for Corrupted System Files

If your PC is up-to-date, then consider whether corrupt system files are causing problems with memory management on your PC. You can quickly check your PC for corrupted system files using the sfc command, which you can run from an elevated command line or PowerShell terminal.

  • Start by right-clicking the Windows start menu and clicking PowerShell (Admin). In the open PowerShell window, type sfc /scannow and hit enter.

  • Wait for the process to finish—this may take a few minutes, or longer, depending on your PC. If any errors are detected, these will be fixed (where it’s possible to do so).

If there are no issues with your Windows installation detected, you can verify that your drive has no file system errors using the Check Disk utility (chkdsk).

  • In the open PowerShell window, type chkdsk /r and hit enter. You’ll be asked if you want to run this check when you next reboot—press Y on your keyboard to confirm, then restart your PC.

Once restarted, your PC should begin the check of your hard drive, repairing any issues it detects.

Reset Windows 10

Software checks aside, a corrupted PC can sometimes only be resolved by resetting or reinstalling Windows to return it to a blank slate, where any unknown software issues are wiped away. This is a last resort, but it may be your last option before you consider replacing your hardware completely.

Thankfully, Windows offers a built-in reset facility as an alternative to a full reinstallation, although you can reinstall Windows entirely using a USB drive or DVD if you’d prefer.

  • To reset Windows, right-click the Start menu and press Settings. From here, press Update & Security > Recovery > Get started to begin the reset procedure.

  • At the next stage, click Keep my files to keep your documents and desktop files safe, or press Remove everything to wipe your Windows installation completely and reset it to a factory default.

Replace Your Hardware

Unfortunately, if this BSOD error is caused by faulty hardware, then no software fix or Windows reinstallation can resolve it. If you’ve discovered a problem with your RAM during testing, for instance, your only option is to replace it.

If you’re unsure if your RAM is faulty, but the Windows Memory Diagnostic Tool has detected issues, download and run MemTest86 to perform a more thorough test of your RAM to confirm if the errors are down to hardware failure.

Your system memory may only be one part of the puzzle, however. If you’re still getting this BSOD error after trying all of these fixes, you may need to replace other components—including your hard drive.

Fixing the Windows Stop Code Memory Management BSOD Error

BSOD errors are rare, but when they come, these tips should help you troubleshoot the issue. The Windows stop code memory management BSOD error is usually a sign of a problem with your RAM, but you may need to run the Windows Check Disk tool to check for problems with your hard drive.

If you can’t fix this error, you may need to look at testing for bad memory on your PC if you’ve run out of software fixes (including resetting Windows itself). Let us know your BSOD fixes in the comments below.

Ben Stockton is a freelance technology writer based in the United Kingdom. In a past life, Ben was a college lecturer in the UK, training teens and adults. Since leaving the classroom, Ben has taken his teaching experience and applied it to writing tech how-to guides and tutorials, specialising in Linux, Windows, and Android. He has a degree in History and a postgraduate qualification in Computing. Read Ben’s Full Bio

Многие пользователи, увидев ошибку memory management, заходят в Интернет и видят, что она называется Синий экран смерти. Человек начинает паниковать, но на самом деле ничего страшного нет. Достаточно много людей сталкивалось с этой ошибкой, и их персональные компьютеры продолжали бесперебойно работать. Если у вас возникла такая проблема, то не нужно бежать в сервисный центр, стоит всего лишь разобрать причины ее возникновения и устранить их.

Что значит ошибка memory management

Иногда эта ошибка возникает из-за каких-то определенных программ, но в большинстве случаев это является проблемой памяти компьютера. Возможно, причиной является оперативная память, а может и нехватка свободного места на жестком диске.

Ошибка Memory Management

Скорее всего, эта ошибка образовалась из-за конфликта операционной системы с оперативной памятью либо памятью на жестком диске персонального компьютера. Выражаясь простым языком, это неправильное распределение памяти при запуске различных программных компонентов.

Однако не факт, что причина возникновения ошибки связана с памятью. Память является всего лишь первостепенной причиной, на которую акцентируются в первую очередь. Помимо этого существует ряд различных других причин.

Причины возникновения ошибки

Memory management Windows 10 ошибка может возникать из-за множества причин:

  • вредоносное программное обеспечение;
  • большая загруженность жесткого диска;
  • драйвера некоторого оборудования отсутствуют либо повреждены;
  • старая версия BIOS.

Поскольку причины возникновения ошибки мемори менеджмент могут быть разные, поэтому для каждого случая используется уникальный метод исправления ошибки. Рассмотрим каждый из них в отдельности.

Вредоносное программное обеспечение

Необходимо произвести проверку файловой системы Виндовс на наличие вирусных программ. Многие пользователи используют нелицензионные антивирусы либо не используют их вообще. Одной из лучших утилит для обнаружения вирусных программ является Dr.Web Cureit. На официальном сайте можно скачать установочный файл антивируса и запустить полноценную проверку файловой системы.

DrWeb

Если оперативная память обновляется при каждой перезагрузке компьютера, то вирусные файлы остаются в системе, пока пользователь их не удалит. Поэтому стоит периодически проверять файловую систему на наличие вредоносных программ.

Исправление ошибки без использования дополнительных программ

В некоторых случаях ошибку можно исправить без установки дополнительного программного обеспечения. В операционной системе Windows 8 и 10 присутствует встроенная диагностика памяти.

Для того чтобы открыть ее, необходимо зайти в Панель управления и перейти в раздел инструментов Администрирования. После перехода необходимо открыть Диагностику памяти. После проведения диагностики, в соответствии с результатами, выявляется причина возникновения ошибки.

Возможно, что сама планка оперативной памяти повреждена. В этом случае необходимо заменить ее на другую. Если используются несколько планок, то надо запускать компьютер поочередно, отключая каждую планку. Если при отключении одной конкретной планки перестанет выскакивать ошибка, то она повреждена и подлежит замене. Также может быть, что оперативные памяти от различных производителей конфликтуют между собой.

Дополнительный софт для сканирования оперативной памяти

Некоторые пользователи не доверяют результатам встроенной утилиты для сканирования оперативки и рекомендуют использовать дополнительные программы. Одной из таких программ является Memtest86.

Memtest86

Она более подробно расписывает результаты диагностики, благодаря этому можно точно определить причину возникновения ошибки. Имеет очень простой интерфейс и достаточно малый функционал, поэтому разбираться в этой программе долго не придется.

Проверка жесткого диска

Если вирусное программное обеспечение не было обнаружено, а диагностика оперативной памяти показала положительные результаты, то вполне вероятно, что решение проблемы заключается в жестком диске.

Для того чтобы просканировать жесткий диск, не нужно скачивать дополнительные программы. Для сканирования необходимо вызвать команду Выполнить и с помощью нее открыть Командную строку. В Командной строке нужно написать sfc /scannow Она осуществляет проверку жесткого диска и исправляет все возникшие ошибки.

Проверка жесткого диска

Также стоит обратить внимание на свободное место на жестком диске. Считается, что для нормальной работы на жестком диске должно быть больше 10% свободной памяти. Если места на жестких дисках не хватает, то стоит удалить ненужные файлы. Для этого можно использовать специальную программу CCleaner. Она пользуется большой популярностью среди программ для очистки операционной системы.

Обновление драйверов

Возможно, что при приобретении компьютера либо при переустановке операционной системы пользователь не имел под рукой лицензионных драйверов, поэтому их установка была произведена автоматически. Операционная система может автоматически устанавливать драйвера для некоторых устройств, но это не значит, что они идеально подходят и будут полноценно функционировать.

Драйвера всегда можно скачать на официальном сайте производителя. Но не все пользователи компьютера знают свое «железо» хорошо. Именно для них существуют различные программы, которые сами определяют компоненты персонального компьютера и предлагают обновить на них драйвера. Одной из таких программ является Driver Booster.

Driver Booster

BIOS

Также может быть, что на компьютере стоит старая версия BIOS. Ее всегда можно обновить. Как это сделать, можно узнать из статей Как обновить БИОС с помощью флешки и Как зайти в БИОС в ноутбуке Samsung и как его обновить нашего сайта.

Неопытным пользователям не рекомендуется самим производить обновление BIOS, лучше всего предоставить такую возможность сделать это тем вашим более опытным знакомым или друзьям, которые точно произведут обновление качественно.

Электропитание

Если ошибка появляется на ноутбуках, то причина, возможно, в электропитании. Для ее решения необходимо зайди в раздел «Электропитание», который находится в Панели управления и выбрать «сбалансированный тип».

Заключение

Итак, мы рассмотрели возможные варианты разрешения проблемы для Windows 8 и 10. Не стоит пугаться при возникновении ошибок на вашем персональном компьютере и нести его сразу же в сервисный центр. Все можно решить благодаря полезным информационным статьям и видеозаписям, в которых досконально описываются решения проблемы. С некоторыми из них может справиться даже самый обычный пользователь, который не разбирается в составных частях компьютера. Стоит только потратить немного времени, чтобы предпринять попытки для решения задачи.

Видео по теме

Python Memory Error

Introduction to Python Memory Error

Memory Error is a kind of error in python that occurs when where the memory of the RAM we are using could not support the execution of our code since the memory of the RAM is smaller and the code we are executing requires more than the memory of our existing RAM, this often occurs when a large volume of data is fed to the memory and when the program has run out of memory in processing the data.

Syntax of Python Memory Error

When performing an operation that generates or using a big volume of data, it will lead to a Memory Error.

Code:

## Numpy operation which return random unique values
import numpy as np
np.random.uniform(low=1,high=10,size=(10000,100000))

Output:

Python Memory Error 1

For the same function, let us see the Name Error.

Code:

## Functions which return values
def calc_sum(x,y):
    op = x + y
    return(op)

The numpy operation of generating random numbers with a range from a low value of 1 and highest of 10 and a size of 10000 to 100000 will throw us a Memory Error since the RAM could not support the generation of that large volume of data.

How does Memory Error Works?

Most often, Memory Error occurs when the program creates any number of objects, and the memory of the RAM runs out. When working on Machine Learning algorithms most of the large datasets seems to create Memory Error. Different types of Memory Error occur during python programming. Sometimes even if your RAM size is large enough to handle the datasets, you will get a Memory Error. This is due to the Python version you might be using some times; 32-bit will not work if your system is adopted to a 64-bit version. In such cases, you can go uninstall 32-bit python from your system and install the 64-bit from the Anaconda website. When you are installing different python packages using the pip command or other commands may lead to improper installation and throws a Memory Error.

In such cases, we can use the conda install command in python prompt and install those packages to fix the Memory Error.

Example:

Python Memory Error 2

Another type of Memory Error occurs when the memory manager has used the Hard disk space of our system to store the data that exceeds the RAM capacity. Upon working, the computer stores all the data and uses up the memory throws a Memory Error.

Avoiding Memory Errors in Python

The most important case for Memory Error in python is one that occurs during the use of large datasets. Upon working on Machine Learning problems, we often come across large datasets which, upon executing an ML algorithm for classification or clustering, the computer memory will instantly run out of memory. We can overcome such problems by executing Generator functions. It can be used as a user-defined function that can be used when working with big datasets.

Generators allow us to efficiently use the large datasets into many segments without loading the complete dataset. Generators are very useful in working on big projects where we have to work with a large volume of data. Generators are functions that are used to return an iterator. Iterators can be used to loop the data over. Writing a normal iterator function in python loops the entire dataset and iters over it. This is where the generator comes in handy it does not allow the complete dataset to loop over since it causes a Memory Error and terminates the program.

The generator function has a special characteristic from other functions where a statement called yield is used in place of the traditional return statement that returns the output of the function.

A sample Generator function is given as an example:

Code:

def sample_generator():
    for i in range(10000000):
        yield i
gen_integ= sample_generator()
for i in gen_integ:
    print(i)

Output:

A sample Generator

In this sample generator function, we have generated integers using the function sample generator, which is assigned to the variable gen_integ, and then the variable is iterated. This allows us to iter over one single value at a time instead of passing the entire set of integers.

In the sample code given below, we have tried to read a large dataset into small bits using the generator function. This kind of reading would allow us to process large data in a limited size without using up the system memory completely.

Code:

def readbits(filename, mode="r", chunk_size=20):
    with open(filename, mode) as f:
        while True:
            data = f.read(chunk_size)
            if not data:
                break
            yield data
def main():
    filename = "C://Users//Balaji//Desktop//Test"
    for bits in readbits(filename):
        print(bits)     

Output:

large dataset into small bits

There is another useful technique that can be used to free memory while we are working on a large number of objects. A simple way to erase the objects that are not referenced is by using a garbage collector or gc statement.

Code:

import gc
gc.collect()

The import garbage collector and gc.collect() statement allows us to free the memory by removing the objects which the user does not reference.

There are additional ways in which we can manage the memory of our system CPU where we can write code to limit the CPU usage of memory.

Code:

import resource 
def limit_memory(Datasize): 
        min_, max_ = resource.getrlimit(resource.RLIMIT_AS) 
        resource.setrlimit(resource.RLIMIT_AS, (Datasize, max_)) 

This allows us to manage CPU usage to prevent Memory Error.

Some of the other techniques that can be used to overcome the Memory Error are to limit our sample size of we are working on, especially while performing complex machine learning algorithms. Or we could update our system with more memory, or we can use the cloud services like Azure, AWS, etc. that provides the user with strong computing capabilities.

Another way is to use the Relational Database Management technique where open-source databases like MySQL are available free of cost. It can be used to store large volumes of data; also, we can adapt to big data storage services to effectively work with large volumes.

Conclusion

In detail, we have seen the Memory Error that occurs in the Python programming language and the techniques to overcome the Name Error. The main take away to remember in python Memory Error is the memory usage of our RAM where the operations are taking place, and efficiently using the above-mentioned techniques will allow us to overcome the Memory Error.

Recommended Articles

This is a guide to Python Memory Error. Here we discuss the introduction, working and avoiding memory errors in python, respectively. You may also have a look at the following articles to learn more –

  1. Python IOError
  2. Custom Exception in Python
  3. Python AssertionError
  4. Python Object to String

Понравилась статья? Поделить с друзьями:
  • Что обозначает ошибка 0xc000007b
  • Что означает mbr error 1 press any key to boot from floppy
  • Что обозначает надпись error
  • Что означает le на стиральной машине samsung как исправить ошибку
  • Что обозначает на триколоре ошибка 10